Readers who need explanations of any of the abbreviations used may find them at Section 1 of the Home Page.
26/07/2009 | Swine-flu, Algorithm and Dives | #200 |
07/07/2009 | Swift Linguistic Changes | #199 |
28/06/2009 | Compound or Phrase? | #198 |
25/06/2009 | Orthoepic Jibery | #197 |
17/06/2009 | Which Tone? | #196 |
16/06/2009 | Mousavi and the like | #195 |
14/06/2009 | Alexander John Ellis | #194 |
13/06/2009 | Flapped /r/ | #193 |
12/06/2009 | To Stress or Not to Stress | #192 |
09/06/2009 | Accented or Not Accented? | #191 |
Blog 200 | The 26th of July 2009 |
I recently he'rd a brief television statement by an
authoritative-sounding speaker explaining things about swine flu
to the “great British public” in which he immediately used the very
unsuitably cryptic word — as far as most of us are concerned — algorithm.
He did, it seemed rather hastily, slip in a gloss “pathway” at his
first use of the term but why use the word at’all if a more ordinary
one’ll do? Swine flu’s a funny combination when you come to think of
it. “Swine” a good time ago gave way in general usage to the word “pig”
and now it’s mainly a technical term of zoology. Flu was an originally
colloquial shortening (of influenza) that’s now come to be perfectly respectable in
the way mob was originally the learned Latin mobile vulgus
(the fickle crowd). I s’pose people’ve avoided saying “pig-flu” coz
it’d’ve sounded like a kind of flu that only pigs caut. That was in
fact the meaning of the earliest recorded sense of “swine-flu” in the
OED. I was quite surprised to see quotes for it as a disease of humans so
early as 1976 and 1981 in American use at least.
To return to algorithm, OED1 in 1884 only recorded the word as an “erron[eous] refashioning” of algorism and offered no pron(unciation). The Burchfield Supplement of 1972 still had no pron for it but for algorithmic
gave /ælgə`rɪðmɪk/. OED2 in 1989 gave /`ælgərɪθm/ but
/ælgə`rɪðmɪk/. All the big three pron dicts (LPD, EPD and ODP) have
only /ð/ forms. I expect the powerful analogy of the unrelated word rhythmic /`rɪðmɪk/ prevented /θ/ from being used in it. For logarithm
LPD and EPD agree that /ð/ is more usual than /θ/. ODP doesnt give a
/θ/ variant for it at’all. Perhaps a /θ/ coming before a syllabic /m/ felt to speakers as much
a beginning to a syllable /θm/ as the end of a syllable /rɪθ/ in fact
so much so that it became affected by the very strong tendency English
has to voice voiceless consonants before a voiced consonant. The
syllabication is neither merely /rɪð.m/ nor /rɪ.ðm/ but the two at
once, sometimes termed ambisyllabicity. When it comes to arithmetic, it’s obviously only perceived as /ə`rɪθ.mə.tɪk/ and not even partly as */ə`rɪ.θmə.tɪk/.
When I was listening to the BBC Radio 4 morning programme “Today” one time last week, a theologian chappy of some sort, in a short talk in the “God-slot” as some call it, was making multiple references to the famous Bible story of Dives and Lazarus. The first of these names was in origin a Latin word for “wealthy (person)”. In ancient Roman times it was probbly pronounced [`diːwes] but people in England have by very natural processes come to pronounce it /`daɪviːz/. Until now I’d never he’rd it, as he repeatedly and confidently sed it, as /`diːveɪz/. It’s well over a century now since phoneticians pointed out that most people’s prons of Latin words were pretty different from the way Julius Caesar and company used to say them. Ever since that revelation you’ve had to be prepared to hear at any time all sorts of revisions of the way people say words they recognise as derived from Latin. This is a new one so I wasnt surprised not to find it in any of the pron dicts.
By the way, it’s not only a bit of a milestone to have come to one's 200th blog, but my automatic word counter tells me that this website has now reached the third-of-a-million-words mark. The latest new addition of some substance is 'English Melodics beyond Basic Tones' Item 5 of Section 8.
Blog 199 | The 7th of July 2009 |
In a postscript to one of last year’s blogs, #101 of the 10th of June,
I mentioned my relief that I had just he’rd a radio presenter confirm
my impression that people seemed to be behaving as if the word problem had suddenly become taboo and were almost universally saying issue inste’d. I notice that a recent letter to The Times has suggested a challenging explanation for the phenomenon. It remarked ‘Every
day one hears talk of someone having health issues, marital issues and
so forth. This, of course, is a product of the “no problem” culture in
which all difficulties depend on one’s point of view, and are open to
debate’. I experienced the same reaction to the suddenness with which I began to hear numerous people refer to being bored of
various things. A few years ago I’d’ve told any student that asked me
about it that I didnt think anyone said it — not to my knowledge at
least. You plainly have to be careful about making such pronouncements.
I
reacted similarly to noticing for the first time people referring to
being “between a rock and hard place”: this strikes me as a sadly
graceless and insipid expression. Of course I can understand that, in
these days when study of classical literature has become an activity
only of the very few, it sounds a bit too flamboyant to refer to Scylla and Charybdis but why not “between a rock and a whirlpool”? I suppose the devil and the deep blue sea
has come to sound a bit extravagant too. Even so “hard place” sounds
terribly flat to me, the word “place” being such a feeble choice that
people seem to avoid accenting it. Its use in contrast with “rock”
sounds so clumsy. I remember, on the other hand, when I first re’d The Tempest
and came across the expression “sea-change” I felt it was rather
mysterious and magical. For years now I’ve been cringing on
hearing it as a tiresome cliché. At last, to my relief, the
wooden-sounding new cliché “step-change” has begun to let it rest. The
recently increasingly indispensable “tipping point” I’m tired of
hearing alreddy. Thank goodness the offen rather pretentious-sounding
“cusp” has been given a rest lately too.
I have the feeling that
the rather recent “Thanks, but no, thanks” is verging on becoming a
cliché. Its intonation is an important part of its identity which can
be suggested as I have by some punctuation that I dou’t it often
receives when it gets into print. With its first word not being uttered in isolation and
being given a level tone, the expression is no dou't preferably
analysable as constituting a single intonation unit (a term I prefer to
the recently popular “intonation phrase” which I shdnt be comfortable
applying to a single word). Thus /ˈθӕŋks | bət `nəʊ θӕŋks/ with the
second “thanks”, hardly accentable as such an immediate repetition of
the word, being uttered as the tail to the fall on “no”. If it were
spoken as /ˈθӕŋks | bət `nəʊ ˏθӕŋks/ it wd lose the sharp even brutal
ironic effect it has because it seems to be used most
offen similarly to “Thank you for nothing”. I quite like this
tonological relative neologism which hasnt become a cliché for me up to
now.
These linguistic items that at least seem to appear and
gather strength rather suddenly are perhaps more widely noticed when
they’re non-phonetic. But there have been quite a few phonetic ones.
The most recent example that springs to mind at once was the striking
way that the weak happy vowel
replaced /ɪ/ as the mainstream value in the least regional accent of
England: the change seemed to be clinched during the last quarter of
the last century. It’s my guess that a reverse happy change
from /i/ to /ɪ/ took place in the second quarter of the nineteenth
century. The CV 3½ "ash" mainstream value [ӕ] went rather suddenly
somewhat down-and-back in the third quarter of the twentieth century.
The very front
beginning version of the go
diphthong had a comet-like passage not being mentioned by anyone much
until well into the second quarter of the last century and becoming
markedly old-fashioned by the time the next quarter began. See the Main
Articles section of this site item 3.7.II for much more on this kind of topic.
Blog 198 | The 28th of June 2009 |
This question is of course the equivalent of asking “Is the sequence or combination Christmas concert a phrase or a compound?” English has lots of “invisible” compounds where the two words involved are neither linked by a hyphen nor written “solid”. We also have many combinations that are spoken with the two stresses more appropriate to a phrase than a compound such as Christmas-Day but which you may find hyphenated eg in the great Oxford English Dictionary. This is of course unhelpful to people who may not know what the verbal tradition is for the combination. Dictionaries specifically designed for people without that prior knowledge such as the Oxford Advanced Learner’s Dictionary or the pronunciation dictionaries LPD and EPD (tho not ODP in this case) are a vastly better guides to usage.
LPD gives eleven examples of combinations with Christmas six of which are single-stressed ie compounds not phrases. These are the exceptional ones. If in dou't always presume that the sequence is a phrase rather than a compound. Be careful not to presume that the combination you he'rd spoken by a native speaker of English must be a compound simply because you’ve observed it, even repeatedly, being stressed like a compound with an accent only on its first element. The “normal” second stress may often be omitted by the speaker because we usually suppress stress on a word or synonym that constitutes a repetition. See on this Website §8.1.
This could very often happen to a phrase like Christmas concert if the subject of Christmas or concerts has been raised in the previous part of a discourse, conversation etc. In a case like How did he come to meet Mary? — She came 'with ˋfrends of ˏhis | to a 'Christmas `concert there’s no precontext that wd prevent concert from taking its basic normal stressed form. However, if concerts had been a topic you might hear: ˋThis ˏyear | they’re having a special `Christmas concert. Or, if Christmas has already been mentioned, you might hear We’ll be singing in the 'Christmas `concert | as `well as in the `church ` ˏservice.
The
practice of writers of English is quite chaotic in respect of
separation, hyphenation and solid writing of huge numbers of
combinations. I’m afraid this shd come as no surprise in view of the
complexity of English patterns of stressing. Continuing with
combinations involving Christmas we find that people in Britain, in a way anyone who doesnt already know could hardly have guessed, generally say `Christmas box, `Christmas cake, `Christmas card, `Christmas present, `Christmas tree but Christmas `cracker, Christmas `Day, Christmas `dinner, Christmas `Eve, Christmas `pudding and Christmas `stocking. American
usage inclines to be more variable. Like the general run of British
dictionaries, most American dictionaries are no help with this matter, even
the Webster ones, tho the big Random House is an honorable exception on many occasions.
Blog 197 | The 25th of June 2009 |
John Wells’s blog entitled “a gross violation?” of the 22nd of June 2009 quotes a new example of Simon Hoggart’s Guardian linguistic sneers thus
Another
of Gordon Brown's weird mispronunciations: he says "gross" to rhyme
with "floss"... The word is in common usage, especially when taxes are
being considered, so you wonder if he really does listen to what anyone
says.
Readers of my blogs 49 and 55
could see that I deplored Hoggart’s comments of that sort because, even
if it were true that his version of the word embodied a misconception
that the pronunciation he uses was a common one, it’d be too cheap
a jibe not to be ashamed of. I gave examples of the idiosyncratic
pronunciations that most of us are capable of acquiring quoting some
from half-a-dozen former pri’ministers.
John rietly pointed out that the term gross and its derivative engross are unique among words which are spelt with oss in having /əʊ/ rather than /ɒ/ but also drew attention to the fact that words containing the comparable sequences ost and oth are very much a mixed bag in that respect. However, he was inclined to the same false presumption as Hoggart, and added “Note the inference that can be drawn if you produce a one-off spelling pronunciation: it means you don’t listen to what anyone says ... Not only EFL learners but native speakers too must learn the pronunciation of a word when they learn its written form. Otherwise people mock.” One of his readers asked “Do you mean to say he first met the word in its written form?! For a foreigner, this might very well be the case more often than not, but for a native speaker? The word isn't so rare or highbrow, I'd say”. John’s reply was “that's exactly the inference I drew. Could be wrong, of course - but I see a 14-year-old Gordon avidly reading about bookkeeping or taxation policy and coming across this word for the first time, and in writing.”
I’m
afraid this usage isnt unique to Gordon Brown. I first noted it from
some other Scottish speakers some years ago and I’ve he’rd it from a
well-spoken titled Scottish lady in a television news broadcast within
the last month. Of course it may have quite a restricted currency
within Scotland but it’s certainly customary for some. It’s rather
surprising that there’s been no Scottish reader of the Wells blog who’s
chosen to comment. I may also mention that Lee Davidson, a Scotsman
long resident in England and my colleague in our days at the Leeds
University Department of Linguistics and Phonetics, confirms that the
pronunciation was familiar to him in his schooldays. Lastly, it’s clear
that it’s very much a minority type of pronunciation in the US too
because the online Webster gives only one form for it: nevertheless
Artin’s very full indications in the great edition of 1961 listed a
second version with the same vowel as in its other oss words.
My
main point is as before that we shd all aim to be as tolerant and
charitable as possible about other people’s unfamiliar pronunciations.
Our grossly ambiguous English spelling makes it particularly difficult
for people to resist misconceptions regarding the currencies of
the pronunciations of various words so we shd all tre’d very carefully
on this sort of ground.
Blog 196 | The 17th of June 2009 |
Tami Date remarked to me the other day
“In your last mail, you wrote ˈBraˋvo! which reminded me of the similar case that I heard in a movie recently:
A: He died unexpectedly and that brings up a lot of questions. It’s probably easier when people are just deceased.
B: ˈProbabˋly.”
When he gave it as ˈProbabˋly
he was marking it tonetically. Tonologically it no dout shd be markt
ˊˋProbably which wd be a single complex tone. As to the actual pitches of the syllables, if what he heard was
[prɒbəbli] the three syllables wd ordinarily have been at mid, hie and
low pitches successivly. If he in fact he'rd [prɒbbli] the two
sylls wdve been mid then (hie) falling — only requiring the same
notation [ˊˋprɒbbli].
I see why my ˈBraˋvo! braut to mind something he was taking to involve two tones but Bra`vo is like Raˋˏther in having basically not necessarily more than one tone/accent, tho it can also have a head tone as I wrote it 'Bra`vo.
This has two tones ie Alt plus Fall ie the climax (nucleus) is on the
second syllable and its two constituents are a head tone and a climax tone.
ALD (the Oxford Advanced Learner’s Dictionary) sez (the bits in square brackets are my annotations or explanatory substitutions) at “Rather: exclamation ... also [ 'rɑːˋˏðɜː] (old-fashioned [slangy], BrE) used to agree with sb’s suggestion: ‘How about a trip to the beach?’
As
to the Climb-Fall tone, a single tone of a complex type, fortunately no EAL user ever needs
to cultivate it coz it’s just a fancy variant of the simple Fall.
(Those who may wish to check that theyve understood these names
for tones, can see them explained briefly at Main Articles §8.3.
It’s
not often that one hears a strikingly wrong choice of tone type, and if
one does it’s highly unlikely to be in an unscripted spontaneous piece
of conversation. Anyway, I he’rd one recently that involved the expression at last in the course of a reading aloud of what to us today sounds
rather stilted conversation. It was written over one hundred and twenty years
ago by the great Victorian writer Thomas Hardy in his minor novel The Woodlanders.
No wonder the reader was caut off her g'ard because not only was the
general style archaic but it involved her in reading the expression at last in what is now an obsolete sense. The passage in question was “What would have immediately followed I know not, but sorrow and sickness of heart at last.”
She re’d that final word with a (perhaps high) full-descent falling
tone with which it sounded quite wrong, but which wd be the type
of tone to be most often he'rd on it today. The speaker was
referring
(quite emotionally one gathers from the general context) to the
possibility of a certain very undesirable ultimate outcome. In today’s
usage,
tho the dictionaries arnt much good at conveying the fact, the
expression at last always
indicates some kind of satisfaction whereas a century or more ago it
was at times used in the neutral sense which the expression in the end has. If in the end had occurred inste’d of at last the reader wdve been unlikely to've been trapped into using a falling tone. There’s a stronger version of at last with the same sense of satisfaction namely at long last. A sentence intoned like this Un`happiness| wdve followed at ˎlast or at `last wd tend to sound heartless. To convey a proper sympathy the word last wdve had to carry a normal (ie un-narrowed) finally low rising tone, namely a simple Rise or a Fall-Rise, Un`happiness | wdve followed at (ˋ)ˏlast.
Blog 195 | The 16th of June 2009 |
The hitherto relatively unfamiliar name of the main rival candidate
for the Iranian presidency who was also a former prime minister, Mousavi, under anglicisation contains the vowel sequence
/muːsɑːviː/. If I were
responsible for advising BBC personnel how to pronounce this name, for
me, it wd present a dilemma. The staff of the BBC Pronunciation
Research Unit have a rational policy of recommending reproducing the original pattern of stress in such words of the
national language of Iran, what we used to call Persian and now is
called Farsi — a word more often stressed `Farsi than in the native manner Far`si. It’s observable that at least most BBC Radio 4 newsreaders are aware of and accept the advice to say Mousavi
as /ˌmuːsɑː`viː/. Aside from these Radio 4 newsreaders there’s much
less uniformity. A commonplace example of the kind of thing to be he'rd
was to be noted on the Radio 4 Today program on one day last week. One cd hear presenter Ed Stourton begin an interview by using the stressing `Mousavi.
Then, on hearing the American politician Richard Perle mention the
name, he with sensitive politeness, switched to his interviewee’s
version Mou`savi. They were followed by a correspondent from Tehran who sed `Mousavi agen and afterwards by the newsreader Neil Sleat stressing it as Mousa`vi.
It
seems to be natural for English speakers either not to notice how
people are stressing such words or at least to ignore the matter. The
name of the Iranian “supreme leader” Khamane’i seems rarely to be he'rd even with the original number of syllables leave alone as OBG (the Oxford BBC Guide)
recommends it [xɑːmənəˈiː]. That looks pretty different from
LPD’s (intended?) first version/ˌkɑːm ə ˈneɪ/. I'd prefer to suggest
[ˈxɑːməˈneɪ`iː] or [ˈxɑːmɑːneɪ`iː]. The word’s not in EPD or ODP. At howjsay.com you can hear it spoken pretty clearly as [ˈxɑːmë`neɪ.iː] which is better than their transcription [xɑːmenei].
I
don’t feel any surprise or much regret that most broadcasters don’t
seem to worry about these matters. I was for three years in the sixties
a lecturer in the Tehran University Department of English and I was
very intrested to note how my compatriots working in Tehran pronounced
Farsi names. I was quite surprised with the way they treated some of
them. In particular there was the way they sed the name of a very
agreeable Iranian chap who was a colleague of my friends who worked at
what was then a very lively and active representation of the British
Council. I never remember hearing a single one of them, including those
who spoke Farsi, refer to him as anything else than /ˈӕli
`sæməndəri/, ending it just like the English word legendary. I’ve never ceased to be puzzled by this phenomenon. His name Samandari was [samanda`riː] in Farsi.
There can’t be sed to be any universal tendency to avoid having words ending /-`iː/: we’ve got quite a few of them like absen`tee, bar`gee, devo`tee, dunga`ree, employ`ee, set`tee. It’s true that some words can be he'rd in either of two stressings like jubilee and kedgeree. For refugee forestress is favoured over the Atlantic and endstress here. We Brits’ve given up chim`panzee in favour of chimpan`zee at least more completely than they seem to have over there. Our usual stressing Ca`pri
shows us perversely going out of our way to endstress what Italians
call [`kaːpri]. The only thing I can put our habit down to is that, having such
very large numbers of words ending with a weak syllable /-i/ spelt with
-y, -ay, -ey or -i
and having so very few that end with strong /-iː/, we simply make the
relatively unusual words conform to the main pattern. Of course
it has to be admitted that we’re very wayward about stressing all sorts of
exotic words. We largely stress what on home ground are Bacar`di,
Chech`nya, Des`demona, Guer`nica, Halkidi`ki, Han`nover, `Helsinki, Hezbol`lah,
`Kabul, Maa`stricht, Me`redith, moussa`ka, omer`tá, roco`co, Tol`stoy, Trafal`gar, Wal`lender, U`stinov etc as Ba`cardi,
`Chechnya, Desde`mona, `Guernica, Halki`diki, `Hanover, Hel`sinki, Hez`bollah,
Ka`bul, `Maastricht, `Meredith, mou`ssaka, o`merta, ro`coco, `Tolstoy,
Tra`falgar, `Wallender, `Ustinov etc. That’s what we’re like!
Blog 194 | The 14th of June 2009 |
One hundred and ninety-five years ago today Britain’s first
important modern phonetician Alexander John Sharpe was born. The very
word phonetician seems to
have been first used by him in 1845. He came of well-to-do parents and
began his schooling in London where he was born. At the age of eleven
his surname was changed to that of his mother, Ellis, to comply with
the wish of a wealthy relative of hers who provided him with such
generous funding that he was enabled to devote his life to scholarship
and even to giving occasional financial support to some of the work of others. He
spent three years at each of two public schools firstly Shrewsbury and then Eton
from which he proceeded to Trinity College Cambridge to study, with
outstanding success, principally mathematics but also Latin, Ancient
Greek, French and German.
After
university, during an extensive
tour of Italy in which he became struck by the different sounds of the
language he he'rd in various localities, he developed an intense
int'rest in matters of pronunciation.
His other travels included living for three years in Dresden after his
marriage. He studied the contributions of German scholars to the
phonetic sciences publishing a translation of an important book by one
of them. He became an enthusiast for spelling reform collaborating with
the shorthand deviser Isaac Pitman. He was a mainstay of the London
Philological Society and was a major mentor of Henry Sweet, who called
him “the pioneer of scientific phonetics in England”,
and also of other phoneticians including James Murray. Ellis wrote
sev'ral books on the pronunciation of English culminating in the five
volumes of his On Early English Pronunciation (1869-89).
This massive work, despite its title, delt not only with Old, Middle
and Elizabethan English but also with the contemp’ry pronunciation of
educated people in the British Isles and overseas and, in its largest
fifth volume of over 2,300 pages, with the dialects “in all parts of Great Britain where English is the ordinary medium of communication between peasant and peasant”.
He co-ordinated and personally vetted a body of workers who collected
the dialect materials for him. The work was never properly finished,
lacking any index etc. Even so it was a stupendous
achievement. He died in 1890.
Among the great advances he made
were his attacks on widespre'd myths about pronunciation matters that
were long unchallenged. One such was Samuel Johnson’s
dictum quoted by John Walker and various others “the best general rule is to consider those as the most elegant speakers who deviate least from the written words”. This was echoed by the remarkable phonetician Alexander Melville Bell, Ellis’s contemporary, who sed “the
less difference a speaker makes between accented and unaccented
syllables — save in quantity — the better is his pronunciation”. Ellis’s comment (EEP Part IV p.1159) pulled no punches: From
this ... principle I dissent altogether. Any attempt to pronounce in
accordance with it would be against English usage, and would be
considered, affected, or ‘strange’, in even the best educated society. Adherence
to that absurd idea had vitiated an enormous number of the
representations of unstressed syllables in numerous dictionaries. Another such pronouncement of his was (p.1209): I
have an idea that the professed men of letters are the worst sources
for noting peculiarities of pronunciation; ... they nurse all manner of
fancies...
Daniel
Jones had become embarrassed by the fact that people didnt like
the labels he’d employed for the kind of English pronunciation he used
and described. He’d termed it first Standard Pronunciation and then Public School
Pronunciation.
He turned back to the nineteenth century to the
favourite term of Ellis — whose contemporaries didnt favour it probably
feeling it sounded too old-fashioned even then, like his reference to
English “peasants” — and said in his 1926 revision of EPD “I call it Received Pronunciation (abbreviation RP) for want of a better term”.
After that he stuck with that term and my colleagues seem mostly stuck
with it too. It was mercifully only a technical term for a couple of
generations but towards the end of the last century it unfortunately got taken up by the
media. Some of us these days try to use something less patronisingly
invidious.
Blog 193 | The 13th of June 2009 |
My correspondent Petr Roesel is an enthusiastic “spotter” of
/r/-flapping. That’s the term he uses and it’s what I usually say too:
tho I have to acknowledge that it’s strictly speaking superior practice
to distinguish flaps from taps. I was about twenty years old when, home
on leave from serving in the British army in the forties, I first
really took notice of the subject of phonetics on coming across in my
city lending library a copy of the Daniel Jones Outline of English Phonetics.
In his first editions of this (in 1918 & 1922) he’d used the term
“semi-rolled” defined as “formed by one single tap of the tongue” but
in 1932 he changed to using “flap” as an apparently generic term. So
that’s how I acquired my habit. In 1962 Gimson’s Introduction
came along using “tap”. Genuine flaps I mostly associate with language
varieties that hav really retroflex articulations like Indic ones and eastern
Norwegian and Swedish in words like blå and the name Ola where there’s a really spectacularly exotic [ɽ] — a mainly-regional variant of their /l/.
But
to get on with my topic of tapped/flapped English /r/ and who uses it
and when. In commenting on my Blog 186 in which I referred to my
quotation of two brief fairly clear clips from Bertrand Russell’s Nobel Prize acceptance
speech Petr mentioned “the word <imperative> and the phrase
<number of>, in which Russell uses a flapped version of /r/”. He
may well have been hearing them more clearly on his equipment than I do
on mine but for my part I cdnt decide whether either of those /r/s was
flapped. However, it tends to strike me that the literature on the
flapping of /r/ in GB (England-English not-regionally-affiliated pronunciation)
is rather seriously unsatisfactory perhaps even misleading. The nub of
the matter is that whether an /r/ is to be described as flapped or not
is very much a matter of degree. Jones and Gimson both referred to the
existence of flapped /r/s in non-regional usage in England with no
suggestion that they were at all socially or generationally marked. In
his revision of Gimson, Cruttenden has labelled such usages as
“refined”. His use of that term cunningly avoids saying whether the
refinement in question is admired or deplored. I’m afraid I’m more in
favour of the brutal frankness of terms like “old-fasioned” and “posh”
with its candid ascription of pretentiousness, affectation or display
of self-attributed superiority. The difficulty is that the refinement
is in the ears of the hearer. At any rate the literature is deficient
in not making it thoroughly clear that there are a range of flapped
articulations of different strengths and consequent prominence which
are crucial to assessing the sociological effects.
Whenever I
think of flapped /r/ in English I remember what a whipcrack of a flap
Noël Coward cd produce on occasion. He had plenty of, for me,
borderline and moderate ones. Note his uses of [ɹ ~ ɾ] especially on
words like moral in a recorded interview about his play The Vortex. Speakers with some degree of Scottish accent often display flaps and even trills. James Naughtie, the BBC Radio 4 Today program presenter/interviewer, may pronounce iron
as [aɪɾən] and often trills a word-initial /r/ in a paralinguistic
expressive flourish. I enjoy the variety of his articulations.
Jones wrote in his Outline
(1932 §750) “Many speakers of Received English use a ‘flapped’ r as a
subsidiary member of the r-phoneme; it occurs chiefly in unstressed
intervocalic position, as in very ˈveri, period ˈpiərĭəd, and when inserted at the end of a word...” He considered that people “generally” used the flapped variety in sequences like pair of. In The Pronunciation of English (1958) he added the example of arrive and mentioned that “a few” used flaps “after θ and ð as in three and brethren”.
It can be problematic interpreting just what Jones thaut it was
reasonable to label as a flap. One has seen in other contexts that he
could take account of articulations that many observers might have
dismissed as too subtly different from another to regard as
differentiable. One such case was his insistence that the normal
“RP”/r/ was fricative. Another was his refusal to recognise a
monophthongal allophone of his /ɛə/ diphthong tho his own tokens of it
included plenty which were hardly perceptibly diphthongal. He recommended
Scandinavian and other learners to practise aspirating the /t/ in step etc.
Gimson
(1980) was inclined to think flaps more commonplace. He noted the
approximant /r/ as “frequently replaced by an alveolar tap [ɾ] in
intervocalic positions, e.g. very, sorry, marry, Mary, for ever, following /θ,ð/ three, forthright, with respect and also, with some speakers after other consonants, especially /b,g/, e.g. bright, grow.”
By the time Cruttenden came to review this statement he preferred to
categorise such an [ɾ] as a sociologically marked variant ascribing it
to “Refined RP”. I suppose my use of [ɾ], or even occasional brief
trills, on the phone — for clarity — is a kind of affectation. Heigh
ho! [ˈheɪ ˏˌhəʊ].
Blog 192 | The 12th of June 2009 |
Tami Date has put some questions to me about the stress patterns to be observed in Number 5 of the dialogues from my book People Speaking,
namely 'Problem Guest', regarding the stressings of three phrasal verbs
that occur in it. They are to be seen transcribed phonemic’ly and
prosodic’ly and to be heard spoken on this website at Main Articles §4.1.5
The ones he quotes are:
So I'm afraid of giving her something that'll bring her out in spots (in Line 4)
and
But I believe she comes out in a rash if she touches onions (in Line 7)
and
For goodness sake! Ring up her house and ask them (in Line 8)
He asks
Why
is 'out' in the ... phrasal verbs unstressed thereby causing a sequence
of three consecutive unstressed syllables between 'bring' and 'spots',
and between 'come' and 'rash'. It is possible to stress the particle,
isn't it?
My answer is basically “Why not?”. He’s perfectly right in suggesting that the word out
in both cases may be strest but there's no “rule” of English rhythm
that sez they must be. Whether a speaker chooses to stress them or not
depends entirely on how fluently or quickly he or she happens to wish
to say the sentence. The woman who speaks the first of these sentences
sounds quite unhurried and natural to me but she chooses to speak quite
fluently and that’s why she doesnt bother to stress the word out.
I
suspect that what’s troubling Tami is the thaut that many textbooks and
pronouncing dictionaries may seem to suggest that a phrasal verb like bring out must not be spoken without stress on the adverb out.
The Oxford Advanced Learner’s Dictionary (online) has ˌbring sth ˈout in sth to make sb’s skin be covered in spots, etc.: The heat brought him out in a rash.
That’s
perfectly good advice for the lexical, citational or other isolate
utterance of the expression and may be he'rd in its use within a
sentence (eg It ˈmay ˈbring you `out in something) but dictionary
users have to bear in mind that as often as not a speaker may prefer to
suppress such stresses in various rhythmical contexts.
If
our speaker had felt that she needed to repeat the sentence becoz the
person she sed it to hadnt been giving proper attention to her, she
might of course have felt irritated and so repeated it in a very
deliberate way. Then it might’ve come out with this sort of prosodic
pattern
So I'm a`fraid of `giving ˏher | ˏsomething that'll | ˏbring her | ˏout in `spots
which of course restores the potential stress on out.
We see that the Cambridge Advanced Learner's Dictionary (online) in a similar situation doesnt bother with stress markings (I prefer Michael Ashby’s OALD version) but has
come out in sth phrasal verb If you come out in something, such as spots, they appear on your skin: This heat has made me come out in an itchy red rash.
Of the third example Tami sed
I
think I understand why 'up' in 'ring up her house' is not
stressed. Here the rule of rhythmic accentuation is applied, isn't it?
My
answer here is that this example is exactly like the other two. I don’t
know of anything called by anyone the “rule of rhythmic accentuation”
but I’d be int'rested to hear of anyone who propounds something so
called. The advice I can give on the matter of sentence stresses is
contained in my article Accentuation §8.1 in the Main Articles of
my Homepage where I point out, among other things, that the effect I
call “globalisation” of sentences (or clauses) may, in the process of
making them more tightly bound units, cause various suppressions of
potential stresses.
Blog 191 | The 9th of June 2009 |
In his blog dated 8 June 09 John Wells reported a query he'd
received from Tami Date regarding a CD of a dialog from a Japanese
school textbook containing the following exchange:
Mother: ... will you pick up your little brother?...
Daughter: Sure, I'll pick him up.
Tami sez of the dauter’s response:... it sounded to me as if there was a high pitch or stress on I’ll.
I don't know which term is more appropriate ... if it is the former ...
I take it as adding emphasis to the whole IP, whereas, if it is the
latter, it seems to me there is no legitimate reason for I’ll to be stressed.
The
fact is that to insist on labelling such utterance-initial tones as
vari'bly either heads or preheads is a futile procedure. I see no practical
value in having a specific symbol for a high (level) prehead in a tonal symbology. Such tones are actually completely
ambiguous in terms of accentuation because there is no audible
difference whether a speaker is giving a word a tone in order to accent
it on the one hand or on the other to heighten the general animation of
the expression. I’ve been pointing this fact out to people for many
years but it seems to fail to register with them. At least no
acknowledgment of the fact has appeared in any textbooks dealing with
intonation matters that I know of. I refer readers to my entries 8.1 Accentuation and 8.3 Accentual versus Animation Stresses
on the main division of this website. There it can be seen that the
concept covers not only so-called “high preheads” but also moving- tone preheads.
The
writer who was the first to clearly identify and indeed to explicitly
name “preheads”, the valuable term he coined for them, Roger Kingdon,
recognised not merely the level ones adopted in the
O’Connor-&-Arnold and Wells books but also falling and rising ones
as well. He devised notations for them which have, not very
surprisingly, been little if at all taken up by other writers.
John’s
reply, which was irreproachable, was that such an expression could well
be intoned with fall plus rise or with a high head or a high prehead
followed by a rise. He might’ve mentioned also that, in view of the
punctuation, which by following “Sure” not with a period but with a
comma, tended to suggest that the whole of “I'll pick him up” might
well be treated by a speaker, particularly appropriately in this
context, as a tail sequence: `Sure, I'll pick him up. Compare
`Certainly! which is without a tail apart from the postnuclear
syllables.
John added The
problem is that the answers contain no new lexical or grammatical
items. So there is no non-given item to place the nucleus on. My
view is that speakers dont find such a situation a problem for them
because, altho we can make a useful recommendation to EAL users that
they shd take care to accent only items that are not re-occurrences,
the so-called “rule” not to re-accent re-occurrences is far from
invariably obeyed by native-speaker English users. I certainly dont see
any reason for classifying the sentences discussed as containing
intonation idioms.