Readers who need explanations of any of the abbreviations used may find them at Section 1 of the Home Page.
24/12/2012 | More on Weakforms (viii). | #431 |
11/12/2012 | The GB goat vowel before co-syllabic /l/. | #430 |
01/12/2012 | Another Hardy Poem. | #429 |
23/11/2012 | Superstrongforms and 'actually' etc. | #428 |
10/11/2012 | The Higgins Boast. | #427 |
28/10/2012 | A Poem by Thomas Hardy. | #426 |
27/10/2012 | Weakforms (vii). | #425 |
19/10/2012 | "Received Pronunciation" re-traversed. | #424 |
04/10/2012 | R Dropping(ii). | #423 |
24/09/2012 | Weakforms (vi) BY. | #422 |
Blog 431 | The 24th of December 2012 |
In my attem·tedly alphabetic listing of weakforms (a term briefly
definable as 'an articulatorily reduced word form constituting a
different set of phonemes from the word's full form') I mainly hav·nt
yet gone beyond those beginning with one of the first three letters of
the alphabet. We de·lt last with actually at Blog 428 and before that
at 425 with can, come and bedroom etc, at 420 with because, at 422 with
by, at 418 with able, almost, always, any, anybody and anyone, at 403
with and, and at 397 with only along with the importantly previously essentially unrecognised
we're and nearly.
Something we've not mentioned much so far is the ease with which
weakforms are produced by aphesis /`afəsɪs/, the loss of a weak vowel or a consonant in the beginning of
or constituting the first syllable of a word. This is a familiar process
from the history of the written language with well-known examples like
the weakening of esquire to squire with the latter being establisht as
an independent word. In fully relaxed styles this is perfec·ly common
today among mainstream GB speakers giving, especially closely following
vocalic sounds, items like it's ·bout time.., high ·bove the clouds, ·f
you take my ·dvice, ·fraid so, the ·lectricity's off, I ·kspec(t) so,
·cording to my idea, go ·cross the road, try ·gain, it's not ·lowed (ie
not allowed, sounding exactly as 'not loud' ) also less casually /ɪts nɒt
l̩aʊd/ (with syllabic /l/), years 'go etc. Various words beginning with the spelling ex- as /ɪks/ or /ɪgz/ or / əks/ or /əgz/ are increasingly he·rd with elision of the first consonant of the prefix particularly often with the word exactly which becomes /ɪ`zakli/ etc.
We've mentioned almost and always as recognised to an extent by some
pronunciation lexicographers. In fully relaxed styles their first
element, the independent word all, can readily shed its consonant as
for instance what we may represent informally as aw by myself, aw my
life, aw the time, ·f aw· goes well, aw night long and very commonly aw
right. Altho awtho exists, prob·bly the more usual weakforms of
although are the two with post assimilation of its /ð/ to an ell viz
/ɔːl`ləʊ/ and /ɔː`ləʊ/.
Finally, while we're still dealing with weakforms beginning mainly
with a, b and c, there's a peculiar change we may mention that
has been catching one's attention increasingly in quite recent years.
It concerns the pronunciation of an before notably the words heroic, historic and
horrific. Both an and its alternating form a descend from the Old
English word aan (meaning 'one') which by shortening produced the
weakform an which in turn itself weakened to a
in the context of a
following consonant. These two ended having the strongforms /an/ and
/eɪ/ and the weakforms of them /ən/ and /ə/. By the Early Modern
English period, when you used one of the few words like historic there
were two natural possibilities. You used /ə/ if you selected its form
sounding the initial aitch or otherwise you chose the form with the
aitch 'silent' and then you employed the other weakform /ən/. I suspect
that many speakers experience something of the feeling, which I share
with them, that this slightly uncomfortable hiatus which the form /ə
hɪstɒrɪk/ tends faintly to
suggest something of a pedantic anxiety to sound 'correct'.
Linguisticly less confident speakers, one suspects, first became aware
that most speakers usually plum·t for the version with /ən/ and no
aitch. However, being worried about dropping any aitch — the most
heavily stigmatised ever of all features of English speech — they
elected for /ən/ but kept the aitch. I've also encountered this recently with hereditary and harmonic.
All this has been leading me to remark on the curious further
recent development of the last two or three decades that more and more
speakers seem to be favouring the practice of originally only 'anxious'
speakers who were inclined to make it clear not only that they're not
dropping any aitch but highlighting the fact by using the word an in
its strongform /an/. It's begun to seem that one can't any longer hear
of at least any historic or horrific happening without its being
preceded by /an/ or /ən/ even from a variety of mature speakers such as Channel
Four's Jon Snow, Professor Diarmaid McCulloch and the BBC Television
News Presenter Sian Williams.
Blog 430 | The 11th of December 2012 |
In 2006 Bente Rebecca Hannisdal of Bergen University Norway was awarded a doctorate for her study Variability and change in Received Pronunciation: A study of six phonological variables in the speech of television newsreaders.
This impressive piece of work was based on data from the three
international news channels BBC World, Sky News and ITV News. From each channel she selected ten
presenters, five male and five female. At page 128 she sed ‘I have included only those presenters who have a (Mainstream) RP accent’ adding that the sole criterion for selecting the speakers’ was ‘non-localisability’.
Given that there are not a few items of disagreement between observers
as to what items are ‘RP’ or not, some might’ve been inclined to reject
most of her selections on the grounds of the localisability to the
southeast of the [ɒʊ] diphthong alone. At any rate, I recently
refreshed my recollections of the fortunately still-available-online
pdf of her work.
Unsurprisingly, I’m afraid, tho very regrettably, no information was
supplied regarding the educational or regional backgrounds of her
subjects. They were not named but referred to solely by numbers. These
people are public performers just as stage or film actors are. They
dont even partly hide their faces when they present news programmes and
there’s a good deal of information about them available in the media.
What reasons can there possibly’ve been for her concealing their
identities. It’s true that Jones, Gimson and others wrote at length
about how people speak with not a single reference to any individual,
but isn't it time this senseless mealy-mouthedness was abandoned. It
can surely only be a hangover from the absurd ‘gentlemanly’ Victorian
tradition begun for radio announcers by Reith in imitation of the
over-revered practice of The Times which for so much of its existence didnt credit contributions to their authors.
When writing in such publications as the Journal of the International Phonetic Association
I’ve always felt perfectly free to attribute by name various
pronunciations to individual broadcasters. An example was that on one
occasion, in 1977 at page 30 of its Volume 7 No 1, I listed a dozen
broadcasters who used unorthographic linking r’s. Tho such brashness
didnt provoke a single whisper of protest, no-one else seemed inclined
to follow suit. For instance Peter Roach’s valuable 2004 Specimen of British English was illustrated by the pleasant voice of a lady who wou·d've surely not been particu·ly shy about being named.
Of Hannisdal's six variables three were vocalic and three consonantal.
One of the former concerned the occurrences of the rather back and
fairly open-mid values ‘before non-prevocalic /l/, in words like roll, cold, shoulder,
etc’ displayed by various speakers in realising their phoneme /əʊ/ for which she
followed Wells’s usual [ɒʊ] transcription. Daniel Jones, even in The Pronunciation of English
(1958), a book with many references to London regional speech,
made no mention of any such special variety of /əʊ/ before dark ells.
Within a few years, however, the first edition of Gimson's Introduction to the Pronunciation of English (1962:129) sed ‘In London .. a more open 1st element is often heard before [ɫ] e.g. in roll, dole, cold.’ Wells’s Accents of English (1982 4.2.6 etc) gave a good deal of space to discussing this and what he labelled as ‘the GOAT split’, assigning it to ‘London-flavoured .. Near RP’. By the time of his 1990 first edition of LPD its p.xxi referred to ‘the use of the special allophone ɒʊ before l in the same syllable in some varieties of RP’. At p xvi he had labelled a diagram with ‘ɒʊ near-RP variant in cold’.
This showed some understandable hesitancy about whether its status was
or was not assignable to what he understood by ‘RP’. Cruttenden in his
2008 latest revision of Gimson at page 142 describes the [ɒʊ] allophone
as ‘typical of London Regional RP’.
Hannisdal came up (at page 155) with the rather surprising finding that 24 of her 30 speakers ‘distinguish
categorically between GOAT and GOAL and use the [ɒʊ] variant in pre-/l/
items ... only two speakers (2 and 28) do not have the [ɒʊ] allophone’. She ventured the comment (at page 157) that her findings were ‘a clear indication that this feature should be considered a part of modern non-regional RP’.
It’s going to be int·resting in future to note how far this newly more
seriously to be considered tendency is corroborated by other observers.
Non-native
speakers at this stage wou·d not be well advised to rush to adjust
their performance to incorporate this feature until wider recognition
is accorded to it. The recent remark by a bloggist that ‘Pronouncing kəʊɫd rather than kɒʊɫd is generally considered ɒʊld-fashioned these days’
is extremely premature. I say this in full consciousness of the fact
that Professor Wells supplied testimonial (now sadly soundless) in his blog of the 3rd of May last year that Prince William cou·d use [ɒʊ] so that ‘it’s clear that the ɒʊ allophone before dark l is now a regular [sic] part of proper, pukka, echt, RP’.
I’ve only noticed it as a rarity so far among GB speakers but I shall
have to keep a caref·l lookout for it in future to note how far it may
prove to become widely fashionable. I cert·nly dont feel disposed to
classify it as ‘Mainstream General British’ as yet. It wou’dnt be the
first time I’d noticed Londonisms (including ones that didnt persist)
in the speech of members of the Royal Family and I share Dr Hannisdal’s
preference for a ‘non-localisable’ definition when it comes to GB.
PS:A striking example of user of an /ɒʊ/ is the Etonian former prime
minister David Cameron who sounds markedly London-area in ending a
speech with the word soul as
he sed it on the 23rd of January 2013. One can hear him saying it
courtesy of Kraut’s English Phonetic Blog at the posting ‘Cameron’s
heart and sol [sic]’ of the 26th of January 2013 by inserting ‘Cameron’
into his ‘Search’ box.
Blog 429 | The 1st of December 2012 |
This time the phonetic int·rest is mainly in what pronunciations the
poet might've expected to be used in saying or singing the lyric —
especially the Latin phrases. The poem is a witty, light-hearted,
playful complaint to his maker (Creator, the Almighty or President of
the Immortals as he puts it elsewhere) about the unsatisfactory sort of
life it has been his fate to live. Hardy was, in his maturity at least,
an agnostic but his religious background me·nt a great deal to him.
Here's the poem with free translations of the Latin phrases. Its title
is often given as "So I have fared" with as sub-title "After Reading
Psalms XXXIX, XL, etc." but the latter is its only title in the
Complete Poems of 1976.
Simple was I and was young;
Kept no gallant tryst, I;
Even from good words held my tongue
Quoniam Tu fecisti! [Because Thou hast so created me]
Through my youth I stirred me not,
High adventure missed I
Left the shining shrines unsought;
Yet — me deduxisti! [Thou hast led me thus]
At my start by Helicon
Love-lore little wist I;
Worldly less; but footed on
Why? Me suscepisti! [As Thou hast brought me to be]
When I failed at fervid rhymes,
'Shall,' I said 'persist I?'
'Dies' (I would add at times)
'Meos posuisti!' [My days .. have been thus by Thee appointed]
So I have fared through many suns;
Sadly little grist I
Bring my mill, or any one's,
Domine, Tu scisti! [Lord, so hast Thou ordained]
And at dead of night I call:
'Though to prophets list I,
Which hath understood at all?
Yea: Quem elegisti?' [Whom hast Thou chosen so?]
We've transcribed it below mainly as Hardy may have imagined it spoken
in his mind's ear: there are other equally possible versions at many
places especially when it's sung and as regards the Latin items.
There's a well-known superb setting of it by the incomparable English
composer Gerald Finzi (1901-56) in his glorious Hardy song cycle Earth and Air and Rain.
I've liss·end to two or three recordings and they seem quite closely
agreed on most dou·tful items at least about what Finzi was likely
to've favoured but it's hardly likely that any tradition survives of
what Hardy (1840-1928) wd've made of them.
The vowel that ends each stanza adhered to how in eighteenth and early
nineteenth-century England teachers of Latin woud've spoken the words.
Hardy woud·ve been accustomed to that style in his youth. He wou'dve
cert·nly known later about the schism that occurred towards the end of
the latter century between the revisionists who had accepted the
findings of historical phonetics that they wd've in the Rome of Virgil
been [i] rather than such diphthongs. However, they suited his purposes
for this poem in which a mildly archaic flavour isnt out of place. The
singers I've noted treat the three words scisti, elegisti and suscepisti
in what seems rather to be influenced by the Continental or at least
Italian tradition. In other places some singers at least use the
revised Latin style of today's schools eg singing /k/ for the c in fecisti.
The item one wonders about most has been also treated that way, saying
[di.eɪz]. Hardy wou'dve known it as /`daɪ.iːz/ to quote the OED entry
that hasnt had its pronunciation updated since 1895. That apparently
lingers on among lawyers at least in their term dies non. The expression dies irae, ie 'day of wrath' I'm accustomed to say as /diːeɪz ɪəreɪ/— no dou·t coz of Verdi's Requiem
containing such a movement. LPD gives priority to /ɪəraɪ/. I hope when
OED gets round to these they give transcriptions for the phrases as
well as the headword. Incident·ly, it's quite unrealistic to deplore
the mixings up we do of these sorts: they're inevitable and it's
thautless, not to say reprehensible, to complain about them.
sɪmpl wɒz aɪ ӕnd wz jᴧŋ;
kept noʊ gӕlənt trɪst, aɪ;
ivn frm gʊd wɜdz held maɪ tᴧŋ
kwouniӕm tu fekɪstaɪ!
θru maɪ juθ aɪ stɜd mi nɒt,
haɪ ədventʃə mɪst aɪ
left ðə ʃaɪnɪŋ ʃraɪnz ᴧnsɔt;
jet — meɪ deɪdʊksɪstaɪ!
ӕt maɪ stɑt baɪ helɪkɒn
lᴧv-lɔ lɪtl wɪst aɪ;
wɜːldli les; bət fʊtɪd ɒn
waɪ? meɪ suʃepɪstaɪ!
wen aɪ feɪld ət fɜvɪd raɪmz,
'ʃӕl,' aɪ sed 'pəsɪst aɪ?'
'di.eɪz' (aɪ wʊd ӕd ət taɪmz)
'meɪ.ɒs pɒzu.ɪstaɪ!'
səʊ aɪ həv fɛəd θru meni sᴧnz;
sӕdli lɪtl grɪst aɪ
brɪŋ maɪ mɪl ɔr eniwᴧnz,
dɒmɪneɪ, tu ʃɪstaɪ!
ӕnd ӕt ded əv naɪt aɪ kɔl:
'ðəʊ tu prɒfɪts lɪst aɪ,
wɪʧ hӕθ ᴧndəstʊd ӕt ɔl?'
jeɪ: kwem eleʤɪstaɪ?
Blog 428 | The 23rd of November 2012 |
Henry Sweet in his 1885 book in German which re-appeared five years later translated as A Primer of Spoken English was of course perfec·ly right to draw attention (for the very first time as it happ·ned) to the fact that non-native speakers of English wd importantly improo·ve the naturalness of their performance if they gave close attention to variations in pronunciation that — automatic·ly for native speakers — occurred in normally fluent conversational style to a particular group of extremely common words. Sweet called the words he singled out for attention 'weak forms' which he was perfec·ly right to do. However, subsequent writers have almost universally referred to them as if they were the only examples in the language of words with weakened pronunciations. Most later writers have expanded upon Sweet's list of fifty-one words. It has been gen·ral to describe them practic·ly all as 'functional' or 'structural' as opposed to 'content' words but such forms can't be limited satisfact·rily to only the ones that have been customarily included. None of these writers has ever offered a satisfact·ry definition of exac·ly what they mean by a 'weak form'. They may be noted moreover to vary consid·rably in what they feel able admit to the categ·ry of 'weak form'.
Regarding the topic of the defining of the term 'weakform', here's an alternative wording of my definition of it for the benefit of those who might find it may help them to understand more clearly that it's the only kind of definition that holds water: Where a word exists in two or more phonemically diff·rent forms one of which is not reduced or weakened in articulatory force compared with the word's other form or forms, that form may be termed the word's "strongform" and the other form or forms may (each) be termed a "weakform" of that word.
Most if not all words may be considered on usually relatively rare
occasions to be able to take "superstrongforms" by having what may be
regarded as abnormally forceful articulations of a kind which are not
employed in the word's most usual strest occurrences. These are of two
types, the phonological, where phonemic composition is modified, and
the phonetic where the modifications of the articulation are not simply
changes to phonemic structure. An example of a phonological superstrongform wou·d be /həʊl.li/ as in eg Holy Moses!
where the doubled ell wd augment the word's phonemic composition from
its normal form /həʊli/. An example of the phonetic type may be
found in the common emphatic articulation of the word 'absolutely'
in the form [`ӕpːsəluːtli] ie with devoicing of its usual /b/ along
with relative prolongation of the resultant [p] with possibly a firmer
closure than is usual for the word's bilabial consonant. If this word
is uttered with a normal fairly mildly strest articulation it need
not be categorised as a superstrongform but only as an alternant
strongform of the word. It may well be impossible to positiv·ly decide
whether one or other of the two forms /ˌӕbsə`luːtli/ and /ˌӕpsə`luːtli/
is necessarily to be deemed the
strongform of the word especially if both are employed in alternation by the same
speaker. Another phonetic example is the exclamatory utterance of the word 'God!'
as [kʰ ɒʰ tː ̚] where its final consonant may be given extra-tight
closure which may be abnormally prolonged and very weakly or not audibly released.
Even if released it may not be voiced as in the word's ordinary
strongform. The whole word may also be breathily articulated. Another example is given in our Blog 391 of the word until.
It shou·d be noted that by our definition there is no limitation, as has been the tradition, of the term 'weakform' to only determiners, pronouns, connectives, prepositions and verbs but it may apply to various other word classes notably including adverbs. We take the example of the very high-incidence adverb 'actually'. Let's look at the first form given in each of the major pronouncing dictionaries: these are CEPD ˈӕk.tʃu.ə(.)li, LPD ˈӕk tʃu ̮əli and ODP ˈak(t)ʃʊəli (= OED 2010). They may be alleged to be easiest for the EAL learner to adopt (in the light of their spellings) but they're all conservative almost to the point of archaism. Until Gimson replaced the onset of the second syllable with /tʃ/ in 1978, EPD had always had /tj/ for it, a rare bird now if not a complete dodo. But surely these current versions with two vowels (in any relationship) between the first and last syllables are not at all the predominantly employed form of the word in isolate use, leave alone fluent speech, that these listings must be presumed to indicate (least of all in EPD's presumably explicitly four-syllable type). LPD is the most credible with its three-syllable possibility but surely its clear medial /u/ is less usual than a schwa. I was confident in compiling my CPD in 1972 that the most normal isolate version was /`ӕktʃəli/ (by the way: I felt obliged in my circumstances then to use final /-ɪ/ in such words rather than be taken to suggest that I was encouraging adoption of a strong final vowel as in /`ӕktʃəliː/) and forty years on I'm as confident as ever th·t that's still so. The ODP/OED judgment that /`ӕkʃʊəli/ is a normal, let alone common, GB variant is far from the truth — however common /ʃ/ rather than /tʃ/ versions may be in the US. So much for the (suggestedly current) strongforms of actually.
This very popular adverb is common as a 'filler' meaninglessly
allowing the speaker to think what to say next. This means it receives
quite a variety of weakforms which in fact not very commonly include
LPD's /`ӕkʃuəli/ and /`ӕktjuəli/ the latter of which can surely sound to many of us conspicuously precious these days, tho its /`ӕkʃəli/ is perfec·ly common GB. LPD in addition has a note quite rightly saying There is also a very casual form ˈӕk ʃi.
Both LPD and EPD include what is prob·bly the word's second most
frequent form /`ӕktʃuli/: it's offen not very clear in such versions
whether the medial vowel is /ə, ʊ/ or /u/. Only ODP/OED offer variants
with syllabic ells. These are credible as possibilities but seem hardly
so as common occurrences in normally fluent speech — sustaining a
consonant enuff in this case for it to become syllabic tends to militate agenst
fluency.
Blog 427 | The 10th of November 2012 |
Ben Trawick-Smith is the American founder of Dialect Blog, a "learning site for people who want to know more about English dialects".
He's an actor who's been claimed to've learned how to perform over a hundred
diff·rent accents. On November the fifth he posted this: "I
heard rumors in college of a speech teacher with an exceptional knack
for guessing dialects. He could supposedly pinpoint, within ten miles,
where a student was from. “Ohio,” he would deduce. “About seven miles
west from Akron.” “Bangor, Maine.” “St. Louis. The western suburbs.”
Such legends have floated around drama schools and linguistics
departments for years. All are variations of “The Higgins
Boast,” the claim Shaw’s Pygmalion protagonist makes in Act I: You
can spot an Irishman or a Yorkshireman by his brogue. I can place any
man within six miles. I can place him within two miles in London.
Sometimes within two streets. Ben continued: "Alas, I find that
most such boasts are exaggerations, although more plausible when Shaw
was writing .. Henry Higgins was “speaking” before modern
tape recorders existed, so I don’t entirely buy his claim. .. So while I
think .. geographically precise guesses are possible in some cases, I
find the idea that someone can pull this trick off for the entire
English-speaking world (or even the entirety of America or the UK)
fairly implausible."
A commenter on that blog using the name "Ed" sed "There
were reports that Stanley Ellis, one of the fieldworkers in the Survey
of English Dialects, had this ability. One case that received media
attention was when (apparently) he located 'Wearside Jack' [John Samuel Humble] a
hoaxer who impersonated the Yorkshire Ripper to the village of
Castletown in Sunderland... However, I’m not convinced. I think
that either Ellis made a lucky guess or that the reports got
exaggerated (I can’t find any direct comments from Ellis on the
subject). To locate someone to a village, you must need more material
than that".
Stanley Ellis and I were both members of the staff of Leeds University.
He lectured chiefly on the English language in its School of English and I
worked especially on spoken English in its Department of Phonetics.
After the broadcasting of the notorious phonecall purporting to
come from the serial murderer who became popularly referred to as the
'Yorkshire Ripper', Stanley and I met for a discussion on the matter, both having been approached by the police for advice.
During the fifties he had carried out all of the hours-long SED (Leeds
University Survey of English Dialects)
interviews with the informants in the farthest-but-one-north English
county of Durham. These included
one made at the village of Washington in an area not far from the
western outskirts of Sunderland, the port where the River Wear flows
into the North Sea. He told me that, not only did one of the speakers
exhibit exactly the kind of dialect he had found in that area, but that
he sounded strikingly like the phonecaller in less definable ways — as
he was able
to confirm by playing back the sound recording he'd made of the
informant.
This was indeed a lucky stroke but in no way termable as involving
guesswork. To my knowledge, Stanley never insisted that the speaker
originated from precisely the village of Castletown but he no dou·t did
suggest that, when the police decided to search for possible suspects
in the area, it cd be a suitable starting place. [He'd been
particularly struck by what must've been voice quality features of one
or more people he'd he·rd there but it was nothing that he cd
scientifc·ly define or attempt to record in any ord·nry phonetic
notation.] A few weeks
afterwards, Stanley and I were taken to a police venue where we were
able to tell the assembled detectives working on the case that we were
both of the opinion that the phone call was by a hoaxer. This matter
was disregarded
by those directing the investigation and we were given to understand
that it was wisht that
we shd not disclose our opinions to the press. After about a year my
patience gave out and I decided to accept that embargo no longer, as
can be gathered here.
As to the question of the "Higgins Boast", I don't even think, aside
from my low opinion of his knowledge of phonetics, it's likely that GBS
really believed anyone cou·d place any speaker within even six miles.
He obviously wasnt willing to let the truth get in the way of an entertaining
story, however preposterous. A few localities in the pre-1914 era in which he was writing
might still have had some communities for example separated by a natural feature
such as a river so as to produce the kind of circumstance suggested,
but examples must've been very few indeed. In ref·rence to London, not
only can experts not assign speakers to even large areas but I've not
come across any expert who can reliably assign a speaker to even north
or south of the River Thames. Among people with whom I've discussed
this matter have been some who've spent most of their life in the
London area including Professor John C. Wells of University College
London who wrote in detail on London speech in his highly respected
book on the Accents of English. All agreed with me. Among the thirty or so responses to Ben's post there was one I cou·dnt
improve upon, by the well-known American speech expert Amy Stoller. It contained the
following:"That some people are both especially knowledgeable and/or especially gifted is .. true. But this? “Brought up in Hounslow. Mother Welsh, I should think.” Let us remember that Pygmalion is fiction, and comedic fiction at that. Does it contain large truths? Yes! Is every detail a verifiable fact? Absurd!".
Blog 426 | The 28th of October 2012 |
I dont read a great deal of verse these days but when I do it's
likeliest to be my fav·rət poet Thomas Hardy (1840 - 1928). He was born
near Dorchester the county town of Dorset in the southwest of England.
He wrote a dozen or so mainly tragic novels which became so successful
that he cd afford to spend the later decades of his life doing what
he'd always liked best, which was writing poetry, much of it
characterised by his observations of nature. I've re·d all his novels
but not yet, at least, re·d all his poetry. His collected verse is a
fat volume of a thousand pages. Anyway, I recently came across one poem
that I hadnt noticed before. It's far from among his best but it's
specially int·resting because in it he refers to his speech. Sadly his
voice was never recorded but he actually mentions in this very modest
little lyric the fact that he never lost his southwestern retroflex
"arrs". Here's a copy of it with transcriptions that include making
some sort of shot at conveying the sounds he was thinking of when he
referred also to the accents of people from Scotland, Ireland and of
posh Home Counties types. These last were I imagine who he was thinking
of when he sed 'Middlesex'. Here it is not attempting to guess his
precise accent but showing where he was likely t've had traces of his
'curly' Dorset rhotics.
The Spring Call /ðə ˈsprɪŋ ˎkɔːl/
Down Wessex way, when spring's a-shine,
The blackbird's "pret-ty de-urr !"
In Wessex accents marked as mine
Is heard afar and near.
/ˈdaʊn | `wesɪks ˏweɪ, wen ˈsprɪŋz ə ˏʃaɪn
ðə ˈblӕkbɜːrdz [ pɹɪt tiː `diː ˏəɽɽ ]
ɪn ˈwesɪks `ӕksnts | ˈmɑːkt əz `maɪn
ɪz ˈhɜːrd | əˈfɑːr ən ˎnɪər.
He flutes it strong, as if in song
No R's of feebler tone
Than his appear in "pretty dear",
Have blackbirds ever known
i ˈfluːts ɪt ˏstrɒŋ, əz ˈɪf ɪn `ˏsɒŋ
`nəʊ ɑːrz əv `fiːblər `ˏtəʊn
ðən `hɪz əpɪər ɪn `prɪti `ˏdɪər
əv `ˏblӕkbɜːrdz `ˏevər `nəʊn
Yet they pipe "prattie deerh!" I glean,
Beneath a Scottish sky,
And "pehty de-aw" amid the treen
Of Middlesex or nigh.
jet ðeɪ ˈpaɪp [pɾatiː `diˏᴧɾɾ] aɪ ˏgliːn
bəniːθ ə `skɒtɪʃ ˏskaɪ,
ӕn [pɛte `deɪˏɒː] əˈmɪd ðə ˈtriːn
əv `mɪdlseks ɔː naɪ
While some folk say — perhaps in play—
Who know the Irish isle,
’Tis "purrity dare!" in treeland there
When songsters would beguile.
waɪl `sᴧm fəʊk ˏseɪ — pərhӕps ɪn `ˏpleɪ—
hu nəʊ ði `aɪrɪʃ ˏaɪl,
tɪz [pɹɹə t̞iː deeˏəɽɽ] ɪn triːlӕnd `ˏðɛər
wen `sɒŋstərz wəd biˏgaɪl.
Well: I'll say that the listening birds
Say, hearing "pret-ty de-urr !"
However strangers sound such words,
That's how we sound them here.
`wel: `aɪl seɪ ðət ðə ˈlɪsnɪŋ ˈbɜːrdz
`seɪ, hɪərɪŋ [ pɹɪt tiː `diː ˏəɽɽ ]—
haʊ`evər `streɪnʤərz saʊnd sᴧʧ ˏwɜːrdz
`ðӕts haʊ wi saʊnd ðm `hɪər.
Yes, in this clime at pairing time,
As soon as eyes can see her
At dawn of day, the proper way
To call is "pret-ty de-urr!"
`jes, ɪn `ðɪs ˏklaɪm | ət ˏpɛərɪŋ taɪm,
əz `suːn əz `aɪz kən `siː ˏər
ət dɔːn əv deɪ, ðə `prɒpər weɪ
tə ˈkɔːl | ɪz [ pɹɪt tiː `diː əɽɽ ]
Blog 425 | The 27th of October 2012 |
This series on lexicographically neglected weakform words has mainly
been aimed at functor items rather than weakenings in the general
vocabulary so that I've recently passed over the word bedroom
at its alphabetic place. Even so the fact that it does have weakforms
seems quite worth noting. Praps some speakers use the alternant
/`bedrʊm/
as a weakform but anyway there're perfec·ly common occurrences of a
weakform with schwa employed before vowels and other, prob·bly
more frequent ones, with syllabic /m/. Examples are
/bedrm `slɪpəz/ bedroom slippers and /bedrəm `aɪz/ bedroom eyes. The same applies to bathroom. The compound bathroom mat offen undergoes the kind of simplification (degemination) which regularly turns some more
into /sə mɔː/ resulting in /bɑːθrə `mat/ even for speakers who wd
usually have no schwa in /bɑːθrm/. Some people have /bedrm/ or /bedrəm/
as strongforms. A similar pair may show schwa-less mushroom, eg
in /mᴧʃrm `suːp/ mushroom soup, but schwa favoured in /mᴧʃrəm `ɒmlət/ mushroom omlette. Note that for a word like headroom the semantic force of the room element wd normally be too strong for it to submit to weakening.
The next functor weakform word we come to is the verb form can. As with
words we've just considered we see what I've called OPEN SYLLABLE
PREFERENCE
operating. This requires that, within rhythm groups, before
vocalic sounds the vowelled weakform /kən/ occurs. Elsewhere either
/kən/
or the vowelless /kn/ may happen to be used. Certain prosodic contexts may favour
one or the other. It can offen be quite hard to hear whether or not a
schwa is present. However, it's easy to observe that no schwa is
present
if a syllabic /n/ is at all long: otherwise there'd be the very
unlikely impression of two syllables. Cruttenden 2008:266 properly
gives both forms but without comment on their distribution. ODP, with
its usual minimalist treatment of such matters, gives only /kən/. So,
surprisingly, does
LPD3 but I take this to be a misprint which was intended to show kən
with italic schwa to indicate "optionally omitted". CEPD gives /kən,
kn̩/, the latter, we note, with subscript syllabicity mark, and also, very
surprisingly, /kŋ/ followed by the (actually superfluous) note
"occurs only before words beginning with /k/ or /g/".
The surprise in this last case is because there seems no point in
according recognition to /kŋ/ as a third weakform when what we have is
merely a matter of ord·n·ry predictable likely assimilations possibly occurring. It wdve been equally
reasonable (and similarly unnecessary) to list a variant /km/ as used
before labials. This is a hangover from Jonesian practice that Wells
sensibly didnt follow. One may ask why, in the circumstances, EPD didnt also give the vowelled
variant /kəŋ/ as indeed it does for the prefix con- at eg congratulate. This suggests raising the very embarrassing question of why no pronouncing
dictionary has ever acknowledged the perfec·ly common occurrence of
vowelless forms of the prefix con-. It'd make perfect sense and harmonise reasonably with practice in other cases if all words with prefixed unstrest con- and com- were shown with italic schwa in CEPD and LPD.
Another notable non-functor weakform word is come. Only LPD records its
existence as having the weakform /kəm/ which it classifies as
'occasional'. It even has the alphabetic entry "c'mon", also to be
found in the OED where it's labelled as 'colloquial' and described as
an 'elliptical' form of 'come on' recorded earliest in 1934. They both
give its pronunciation as /kəm ˈɒn/. A sentence such as ‘Can you come up to me or shall I come over to you’ wou·dnt sound atall unusual in casual or conversational GB speech with /kəm/ at both occurrences of come.
The negative contraction couldn't is not recorded as having any
weakform in EPD, ODP or OED. LPD has the very common minor weakform
/kʊdn/ which GB speakers usually avoid at the ends of rhythm groups
(where it's of course particu·ly prominent) which fact is conveyed not
quite satisfactr·ily by the note 'in standard speech used mainly before
a consonant'. The weakness in question can be illustrated by
an exchange like "I cou·dnt ask him — No, you cou·dnt" in which the
first orthographic word-final /t/ cd be elided but omitting the second
wou·dnt sound like normal GB usage: /aɪ `kʊdn `ˏɑːsk ɪm — ˎnəʊ. ju
`kʊdnt./ LPD adds 'This word has no weak form' a view which we've
already dissented from at our Blog 396. Wells very much acknowledgedly
has his EFL readers in mind and it cert·nly wd be of no benefit to them
to concern themselves to adopt the weakform /kədnt/. There do happen to be very casual weakforms of non-final couldn't which drop the /d/ or pre-assimilate it to another /n/ before its existing /n/ giving eg /aɪ 'kʊnn̩ `seɪ/ for I cou·dnt say. More extremely the syllabic /n̩/ cd be elided leaving only a single simple /n/ except pre-pausally.
Blog 424 | The 19th of October 2012 |
In Historical Linguistics of English: An International Handbook,
edited by Professor Alexander Bergs of Osnabrück and Professor Laurel
Brinton of Vancouver and publisht from Berlin by Walter de Gruyter,
there appeared in July a very substantial article (well over 7, 000
words) from Oxford Professor Lynda Mugglestone entitled 'Varieties of English: Received Pronunciation'. It "explores the history and identity of ‘Received Pronunciation’ .. from the 18th century", (when talk of a non-localized British accent, it's suggested, first appears) "to contemporary discussion of both usage and attitudes", including what she calls "crises of definition and identity. New archive material" offer·d takes the form of quotations from the sev·ral unstartling unpublisht documents she's examin·d at the BBC's "Written Archives Center"
[her editors seemingly require a transatlantic rather than the BBC's
own spelling]" near Reading in the process of attempting to "evaluate the question of its continued validity, either as label or linguistic reality".
This is a reprise of the topic of the final chapter of her 360-page book "Talking Proper. The Rise of Accent as Social Symbol" publisht in 1995 and revised in 2003 with the addition of that important extra chapter on "The Rise (and Fall?) of Received Pronunciation". The present article begins by citing comments on the 'identity and role' of RP, remarking that 'its claims as automatic reference model in dictionaries and in foreign language [sic] teaching have .. recently been contested'. She quotes Abercrombie's 1951 remark that the existence of RP "is an anachronism in present-day democratic society" giving its date as if of its 1965 reprinting. She adds that "Roach later concurred".
Misquoting the page (as viii when it shdve been 3) on which in his
well-known EAL textbook from its third edition of 2000, she sez Roach
referred to (actually only the name, it isnt made totally clear) Received
Pronunciation as "old-fashioned and misleading".
The current 2008 edition of that book repeats the opinion at its page
3. A few lines later she quotes that Roach book again, this time
getting the page (if not the wording) right, as saying of EAL teaching,
“If we had a completely free choice of model accent it would be possible to find more suitable ones”. Absolutely unjustifiably, she claims that thus he 'argues, advocating Scottish or Irish accents instead'.
This overlooks his immediately following perfectly clear
acknowledgment, after saying what he thaut coud've been their
advantages (presumably in some ideal world), that such a move was "not a practical possibility".
He was 'advocating' nothing of the sort. Nor has to my knowledge anyone
else really seriously done so, leave alone produced related materials.
She next remarks that some writers continue "foregrounding both social evaluation and supraregionality as salient elements in its construction".. adding that others "contest the viability of socially orientated (and especially class-based) meanings". She then turns to saying "Elsewhere the demise of RP is predicted .. in favor of [what is] widely labeled Estuary English" quoting Przedlacka's (2000) justified dismissal of it as merely "a putative variety .. located in the Home Counties".
In what at this stage constitutes an aside, she turns to the origins of
the term RP rightly devoting attention to the influences of Henry Cecil
Wyld and Alexander John Ellis. She makes it clear that Daniel Jones
didnt quite exactly take over RP as a ready-made term, pointing out
with commendable precision that Ellis "was
.. first to deploy the initialism by which Received Pronunciation would
often later be known: “rp., received pronunciation, or that of
pronouncing dictionaries and educated people” ". After this she returns to saying "The
search for a less loaded, more neutral, designation for the speech of
those who do not reveal their regional origins by means of accent has,
as we have seen, prompted a proliferation of alternative labels." On coming to Roach's term 'BBC (English Pronunciation)' she effectively dismisses it with "in
an era in which the BBC is actively extending the accents used upon the
airwaves, including in authoritative domains such as the news (and the
traditional bastions of RP, on BBC Radios 3 and 4), the label “BBC
pronunciation” can also create problems." Then she continues, quoting Roach 2004 from JIPA. Apparently treating as fact his questionable claim that “[t]he
number of native speakers of this accent who originate in Ireland,
Scotland and Wales is very small and probably diminishing, and it is
therefore a misnomer to call it an accent of BRITISH English”, she claims that your bloggist's "favored “GB” (“General British”) is equally liable to dissent."
This last comment seems misinterpretation of my definition of General
British as a pronunciation label. When I first introduced the term and
its initialism GB in the first issue of my (Oxford) Concise Pronouncing Dictionary of British and American English
in 1972 I equated it (at its p.xiv) precisely with Gimson's terminology
of the kind 'the general Received Pronunciation of British English'.
For the past century at least, the term British normally excludes
Irish, ie includes only England, Scotland and Wales. In my opinion,
very substantial numbers of the inhabitants thruout all three countries
whose daily speech is non-dialectal ('Standard') English employ
exclusively or mainly General British pronunciational usages. This, I
submit, makes the term "General" British pronunciation a very feasible
one.
I do not consider the terms 'Non-Regional Pronunciation' or 'BBC Pronunciation'
grossly unacceptable but I feel that General British/GB has advantages
they both lack, notably of conciseness and explicitness regarding
geographical location and diffusion. Of these two serious rivals, I
have the greater sympathy with the former because it lays emphasis,
where I feel it shou·d best be placed, on the geographical diffusion of
the accent while avoiding any invidious and/or questionable references
to its sociological status. I think its aim might perhaps have been
more succinctly achieved by adopting 'neutral' rather than
'non-regional'. Its initialism 'NRP',
by incorporating the two letters of the item it replaces (tho the 'r' doesnt
stand for the same word), might well cause slight unease if not
confusion in some quarters. It also lacks part-of-world identification.
What is me·nt by 'BBC Pronunciation'
is likely to be quite widely understood because of its history as an
informal term among British people. As a geographical identifier it is
effective but not free from cert·n objections including that the term
has never been recognised, let alone adopted, by the BBC itself. From
the sociological standpoint it perhaps carries something of the
disdvantageous baggage of 'received'. Its employment has to be prefixed
with a caveat that, altho in its earlier days liss·ners to the BBC cd
be confident of hearing "BBC Pronunciation"
from its staff speakers, over the past half century this has become
increasingly not the case even from the Corporation's overseas
services. It may well be felt by some that its history of markedly
informal popular use must to some extent tend to taint its serious
application in the vocabulary of linguistics.
There is one other serious contender which needs to be mentioned
because it has obviously gained in currency of recent years as a
consequence of its appearance since 1999 in the Handbook of the International Phonetic Association. When Jones edited (anonymously) the 1948 booklet entitled The Principles of the International Phonetic Association he included among the fifty or so examples of the alphabet's use a Specimen of English which he subtitled in brackets "(One variety of Southern British)". This he was ord·n·rily elsewhere at the time disposed to refer to as 'Received Pronunciation'
but one may presume that he was observing a tradition that the
Association didnt express preferences for rival terms for items such as
language varieties. When Professor Francis Nolan fulfilled admirably
the main responsibility for the editing of Part 1 of the Handbook he was presumably following this custom when at page 4 he referred to the "two varieties General American and Standard Southern British".
Unfortunately this formerly very little used expression and its
initialism SSB(E) include two very questionable choices of word. The
first problematic term Nolan himself identified as being so by his
bracketed admission "(where 'Standard' should not be taken as implying a value judgment of 'correctness')".
It seems far better to avoid a word altogether rather than to have to
instruct readers not to interpret it in its normal meaning. The other
unsuitable choice was the word 'Southern'
which wd serve perfectly well to describe the ultimate historical
sources of the variety but c·n only be deplored as a choice when one
considers the very consid·rable numbers of speakers born and bred in
the north of England and in Scotland and Wales who are native speakers
of the variety.
Blog 423 | The 4th of October 2012 |
Our Blog 385 promised a return to the topic of /r/ elision. I hinted
then at the absence of adequate coverage in pronunciation lexicography
of words in one notable group mamely of words containing an earlier
syllable beginning with the cluster /pr/ or /fr/ from which its /r/ is
lost when another /r/ follows in a later syllable of the word. Such
dropt /r/s seem to be particu·ly tho not necessarily prompted when the
later /r/s occur in the onsets of strong syllables. So words
like preference and prosperous arent to be expected to lose their latter /r/s of their first syllables. Those which are offen found to lose them include preliminary,
prerogative, prescribe, prescription, prescriptive, priority,
procrastinate, program, programme, progress, progression, progressive,
pronounce, pronunciation, proprietary, proprietor, propriety,
propeller, propose, propulsion, proscriptive, protractor, protrude,
protrusion. The word programme
is particu·ly easy to observe becoz it may be he·rd very frequently
all round the clock from all manner of broadcasters. It's most offen
/`pəʊgram/ but also very offen /`prəʊgam/ and, even so, quite
frequently /`prəʊgram/. Only very occasionally does one hear /`pəʊgam/
from speakers who're particu·ly relaxed or particu·ly in a hurry— but it does turn up now and
then.
The only common items with the much less frequent cluster /fr/ are frustration, infra-red and infrastructure which last seems to chiefly take the elided form [`ɪɱfəstrᴧkʧə]. The other two one feels less confident to comment on coz they
too infrequently crop up to be very sure about but infra-red apparently fairly offen drops an /r/. LPD's only variants of these
words are ones having slightly surprisingly epenthetic /t/. Neither CEPD nor ODP has any
r-dropping variant of this or any of our other examples so far. Cert·n
words show somewhat sim·lar behaviour eg entrepreneur which is prob·bly most offen /ɒntrəpə`nɜː/. The word protuberance is sometimes pronounced /pə`truːbərəns/ by the not inconsiderable numbers of speakers who confusedly connect it with protrude. The term spectrogram
offen shows a comp·rable anticipative pattern to the ones described
above by losing its first /r/, among phoneticians at least, and so becoming /`spektəgram/. Those who
might imagine that Scottish speakers pronounce ev·ry r
of the spelling may care to note that not on·y is that very far from true but that they even may drop /r/s that not all
speakers in England elide. For example the Scottish politician Alex Salmond sez /pə`skrɪpʃn/.
Turning from dissimilatory types of /r/ elision to others, one may
mention a couple of rhythmic·ly complement·rily distributed extreme
weakforms of from /fəm/ and /fm/ ie [fm̩]. Perhaps,
besides its weakform /praps/, has the even weaker and more informal but
not uncommon /paps/. I he·rd it the other day from the admired BBC
Radio 4 presenter of the Today programme Sarah Montague — unscripted of
course. Just after I'd finisht writing this I happen·d to return to a podcast from the BBC radio series The Life Scientific
in which Richard Dawkins was the interviewee. In one of his first few
sentences he sed '...it turns out that /paps/ that wasnt quite right..'
It's always a pleasure to provide an example that readers can check out
for themselves.
By the way, the simplification of the /fr-/ cluster by dropping of its /r/
is to be seen in the historical development of the feminine forename Frances which having been shorten·d to Fran was afterwards familiarised to Franny and later-still reduced to Fanny.
Many /r/s are dropt medially in polysyllabic words with no particular
reason other than the rhythmic pressure to utter the item in a manner
that fits it cumftably into its sentence. None of the
three major dictionaries give recognition to the common forms /`tempəʧə/ for temperature or /`sekətri/ for secretary. Merriam-Webster Online in an unusual departure from its accounts of American pronunciations comments on one of our usages saying "especially British \ˈse-k(r)ə-trē\".
Wells accords /`sekətri/ the LPD warning sign ⚠ labelling it as
'considered incorrect'. The other two offer no such information. Graham Pointon, the former head of the BBC
Pronunciation Research Unit, in his Linguism blog at the 19th of December 2008 sed int·restingly:
"After February, secretary must
be the word in which [r-dropping] caused the most complaints at my time
at the BBC, when any perceived ‘dropped’ /r/ would bring letters on to
my desk. And yet veterinary
is accepted without question by almost everybody when it is pronounced
/ˈvet(ɪ)n(ə)ri/. No one ever complained about it in my 23 years of
answering such letters".
I shd sooner feel like writing in complaining if anyone'd committed the tiresome ped·nt·ry of saying /vetərɪnəri/. Veterinary as /`vetnri/ gets into LPD without a cautionary comment. It's not recorded in EPD or ODP. We de·lt with February in our Blog 385. Graham also mentioned arbitrary, contrary, library, and literary as losing /r/s. I gave each of these in my Concise Pronouncing Dictionary with /r̩/ to be understood as readily losing its syllabicity which is what gen·ly (ie generally,
with a common r-drop from /ʤenr̩li/ which looks more unusual than it
sounds but isnt in any dictionary) happens to them in fluent speech.
None of the three pronunciation dictionaries accepts any /r/ loss from deteriorate,
LPD 'triangulating' specifically /dɪ`tɪərieɪt/ and even /dɪ`tɪərəreɪt/
which, having a yod-drop but no r-drop, seems likely get by unnoticed
by most folk. Another problematic word is respiratory
which can be he·rd at times as /`respɪtri/. I've noted it from at least
one BBC news presenter (Ben Brown) but that's too far from its accepted spelling to be likely to achieve
respectability. The LPD poll of self-appointed judges favoured re`spiratory with tonic vowel /ɪ/ for the word.
Blog 422 | The 24th of September 2012 |
By (the preposition) etc
If you shou·d be reading this exclusively for practical gi·dance on how
to use spoken English you need go no further than the typical very
sound advice we shall quote from the LPD (the Longman Pronunciation Dictionary), "The EFL learner should always use the pronunciation baɪ."
Any people who find the topic of weakforms int·resting in itself, may
wonder why this word has no weakforms — or at least none we're
recommended to adopt — whereas there're five other common prepositions
that have plenty. Users of spoken English who have the desire to sound
'natural' to native speakers when conversing with them wou·d be very
ill advised to neglect any of those five (at, for, from, of & to).
The ODP (the Oxford Dictionary of Pronunciation for Current English),
which is gen·rally economical in its treatment of EAL (English as an
Additional Language) matters, understandably mentions nothing about any
weakform. On the other hand both the LPD and the EPD (the Cambridge English Pronouncing Dictionary) record the existence of what they both call "occasional" weakforms of by.
The first of these is shown as \bɪ\ by EPD and \bi\ by LPD in which \i\
is a cover symbol used to avoid repeatedly giving all of the three possibilities
specifiable in a notation nearer to phonemic as /iː, i & ɪ/. This
\i\ stands for a normally markedly shorter allophone
of the /iː/ phoneme than the one that occurs when that phoneme receives a lexical
pronunciation.
The two main dictionaries agree in representing a second weakform of by as /bə/. LPD slightly ungrammaticly refers to "an [sic] occasional weak form bi, bə which is [sic] stylistically marked and .. restricted to set phrases". EPD gives an example of such a phrase which it sez occurs "particularly in measurements (e.g. 'two by three' /ˌtuː.bə ˈθriː/". Other phrases include 'fly-by-night'
at which OED3 sez it is "not yet..fully updated"
and doesnt supply
the pronunciation one hopes will be included eventually in the fully
revised version. The most commonly found meaning of the expression it classifies as 'slang' and defines as
'one who defrauds his landlord or creditors by decamping in the night'.
Obviously the editors are far too gentlemanly to phrase the definition
in a form that wd encompass the possibility of such a practice on the
part of the fair sex. Those many who at least at one time needed to clean their rifle
barrels were required to employ for the purpose a piece of cloth known as a
'four-by-two' called by all ranks in the British Army a /fɔː bə `tuː/.
There's an an·ser to the question "Why arnt there lots of weakforms of by?" In fact there were plenty of them in Middle English and even in Old English (Anglo-Saxon). In a sense there are some of them still but, being so offen in fixt phrases, they've lost their independence and been captured by following nouns which have caused them to be spelt prefixed to them. This has resulted from eg earlier by cause of becoming because of /bi`kɒzəv/ where the other preposition of has managed to resist capture. In correct written usage today a contraction like "becoz've" isnt (yet?) an accepted spelling. Outside of fixt phrases like the ones with two numerals we've quoted, the weakform /bə/ on·y seems to occur in phrases that tend to be sed quickly and/or carelessly such as a beginning like "By the time that I..." or "So I grabbed him by the scruff o' the neck..." and so on.
Bye
the farewell has no weakform tho it's subject to strest allophones
Bye-bye the farewell has the variants /ˈbə ˈbaɪ/ and less predictably a version which CEPD records not unreasonably as /ˈbʌ ˈbaɪ/ but which LPD doesnt mention