Readers who need explanations of any of the abbreviations used may find them at Section 1 of the Home Page.
07/06/2013 | Another Idiosyncratic Pronunciation. | #451 |
27/05/2013 | English Urban Winter PS 23 | #450 |
10/05/2013 | Derby and Similar Words. | #449 |
01/05/2013 | Weakforms (xi). | #448 |
27/04/2013 | John Baldwin. | #447 |
25/04/2013 | A Comprehensive New Edition. | #446 |
23/04/2013 | The Wells Standard Lexical Vowel Sets. | #445 |
23/04/2013 | A Text-to-Speech Facility. | #444 |
14/04/2013 | A Notorious Estimate. | #443 |
10/04/2013 | IPA Transcription 1900. | #442 |
Blog 451 | The 7th of June 2013 |
The surname name Boleyn as
pronounced in a recent BBC television program presented by a Professor of
the History of the Church in the University of Oxford has been a puzzle
for my fellow bloggist John Maidment. Once agen, rather than riding at
excessive length on the back of your post on the matter, John, I offer
you and others who may be int·rested a bit of discussion on the topic
in a post of my own.
As that of a Queen of England, Anne Boleyn's name might've praps been
presumed t've come down to modern times in a relatively undisputed
spelling and pronunciation. Extensive and reliable records of the
pronunciations of 'proper' names ie those denoting a particular person or
place etc are harder to come by than those to which we apply the other
grammatical technical term 'common'. In the 70s and 80s of the
nineteenth century, the (British) Philological Society, of which I've
been a lowly member for sev·ral decades or so, in planning its ambitious New English Dictionary on Historical Principles (whose title was later to morph into the snappier Oxford English Dictionary)
came to the momentous — and to my feeling most regrettable — decision
to exclude from their coverage anything but 'common' words. By the time
the OED had barely got underway an enterprising American publishing
house had produced an excellent work, entitled The Century Dictionary of the English Language, in se·vral volumes which was followed fortunately by a stablemate The Century Cyclopedia of Names. This last in 1899 gave "Boleyn or Bullen" with the pronunciations /`bʊlɪn/ or /`bʊlen/ (the latter of which looks dubious).
In the twentieth century Daniel Jones, along with Hermann Michaelis, his co-author of their curiously arranged 'Phonetic Dictionary of the English Language' of 1913 (they awkwardly alphabetised its entries by their phonetic not their ord·nry spellings), gave for Boleyn /`bʊlɪn/ and /bʊ`liːn/. In 1917 Jones, in the first edition of his English Pronouncing Dictionary, added an extra alternative showing /`bʊlɪn, bʊ`lɪn & bʊ`liːn/.
These were never changed by Gimson when he took over the EPD but only
by Roach et al eighty years later in the 1997 so-called fifteenth
edition of EPD which gave the sev·ral versions /bə`lɪn, bʊ-, bəʊ-;-`liːn; `bʊlɪn/.
In 2011 in the re-chriss·ened 'Cambridge' EPD the last of these was
withdrawn. (I'm only giving British usages. It remained as the first
US entry in this now 'C'EPD). Wells's LPD has always listed three (British)
forms /bə`lɪn,`bʊlɪn & bʊ`liːn/.
The name has also come down to us in the forms Bollen and Bollin the latter, at least, unsurprisingly pronounced by its owners as /`bɒlɪn/
as LPD testifies. Surely this is what accounts for Professor (Sir)
Diarmaid MacCulloch's choice of his unusual pronunciation of Boleyn.
From a Cambridge PhD in Tudor history this pref·rence is not to be
received without due respect but one wishes that he'd acknowledged and
explained, so justified, using such an idiosyncratic version. Cert·nly
it's a perfect possibility
that Anne's name cou·dve come down to us gen·rally pronounced as /`bɒlɪn/
but it obviously hasnt done so which makes his choice seem so odd
to the rest of us. By the way, MacCulloch's Gaelic-originated forename, as they
so offen do, occurs in various spellings and pronunciations. I wasnt
liss·ening hard for it but it may've been announced as /dɜːmɪd/. None of
the usual sources lists it; CEPD has the nearest on offer with Diarmuid as /`deəmʊd/.
Perhaps the most surprising thing about the name Boleyn is that no-one seems to pronounce it /bə`leɪn/. It was cert·n t've existed in that variant form in Anne's day. The comparable variant spellings of Allen as Alleyn and Alleyne now correspond to the forms /ӕ`leɪn & ӕ`liːn/ as LPD testifies. Shakespeare in one of his sonnets stresses 'barren'
on its second syllable (the one beginning with 'When I doe count the
clock that tels the time' #12 in the Oxford Original Spelling edition
of his works at page 850 'When lofty trees I see barren of leaues') so
we neednt be surprised to find that in other people's works in his day
it's found in the spelling barraine. Boleyn comes ultimately from the French placename Boulogne. E. J. Dobson's massive English Pronunciation 1500-1700 Volume II §272 gives numerous examples of words with the same sorts of vowel variation as we see in Bullen alongside Boleyn ie of simple 'e' varying with dual 'ei' or 'ai' etc. Words like bargain, Britain, captain, certain, curtain, foreign (whose 'g' isnt found before the fifteenth century and was apparently a pedantic insertion by association with 'reign' where one did belong originally), fountain and villain have become the sort of 'grapho-phonemic misco·ordinations' that were the subject of our Blog 153. They show pronunciations persisting that are now mis-matched to the spellings we've kept. Compare also (cousin- etc) german and germane, Roman and Romaine (which at least survives as a surname) and travel and travail. Dozen in the fifteenth century cd be dosein, dosseyne etc. At villein OED gives the two pronunciations /`vɪlən & `vɪleɪn/ saying "Both types of spelling [ie villain and villein]
have been freely employed for this special sense [serf] of the word,
and the tendency to use the form villein has increased in recent
years". So has the latter pronunciation as well they might add.
In the same BBC Tudor series an enjoyable account of the life of the Bible translator William Tyndale presented by the polymathic (Lord) Melvin Bragg seemed to suggest some comparability with the above. Those who take the precaution of checking what might be the traditional pronunciation of Tyndale's name will find that CEPD and the Upton-et-al Oxford DP give only the counter-orthographical /tɪndl/ for the name but that LPD admits the subvariant /`tɪndeɪl/. The fact that sev·ral times Bragg slipped into this spelling-harmonious form suggests that he might well've adopted the counter-orthographical form after previously being inclined to take the name as pronounced otherwise — which wd've been perfectly understandable. Tyndale or Tindale isnt a very common name and the only possessor of it I've ever known is a long-time fr·end and ex-colleague who pronounces it /`tɪndeɪl/. One is inclined to wonder whether there's any evidence regarding what pronunciation the Bible translator himself used, not th·t that need be necess·rily of overwhelming importance in regard to one's personal decision on how one prefers to say it.
Blog 450 | The 27th of May 2013 |
Dialog 19 of my book People Speaking is 'The Rigours of the English Summer'.
Readers are recommended to go to the #19 sound file at Section 4.1 of
the main division of this website. Ideally, if possible, transfer the contents of the sound-file onto Audacity (the
free audio facility) to be able to select convenient slices for repeated
playback.
1. Dreadful weather, isn't it?
/ˎdredfl ˎˏweðər ˎɪznt ɪt./
Notice that she expressively makes the initial affricate /dr/
distinctly longer than it usually is. Also that, tho this kind of
sequence of statement and tag question might well occur as two sep·rate
rhythm phrases, even with a slight degree of pause between them, here
they're smoothly linkt by the "optional" final /r/ used at the end of
'weather'.
The rather low pitches express her disgruntlement.
Please remember that our tone markings represent only very approximate, not at all precise, pitch values.
2.— Huh! Terrible! Can't remember such a rotten June.
[ˎhᴧh] `teːrəbl. ˈkɑnt rᵻ`membə səʧ ə rɒtn ʤuːn./
Conventionally writers gen·rally use wording such as 'oh' or 'ah' when
the expressive noise a speaker makes isnt a normal phoneme-sequence
word. Here for this breathy [hᴧh] no dou·t 'huh' does very well. In fact the lexicographicly recognised exclamation 'huh' is
usually found in this sort of context expressing what OED calls "some
suppressed feeling".
Altho this transcription is mainly phonemic we show various noteworthy
length features as eg here where the /e/ of 'terrible' is so stretcht that
the word becomes homophonous with 'tearable' /`tɛːrəbl/. We've shown the
first vowel of 'remember' as not safely identifiable as either /ɪ/ or
/ə/ by using the strictly-speaking unauthorised IPA symbol [ᵻ] for a
vowel in between the two phonemes. Note that 'such' is used in a
weakform which CEPD and LPD consider to be only 'occasional'. It's a
relaxt-style feature.
This speaker turn ends with an exceptionally long low intonation tail.
This sort of thing is mostly avoided so as not to sound too gloomy but, of course,
it's completely suitable in this context.
3.— Nor can I.
/ˎnɔː kən ˎaɪ/.
4.— I feel sorry for all those poor devils
/ `aɪ fil ˏsɒri | fər ɔːl ðəʊz ˎpɔː ˏdevlz |
taking the only couple of weeks' holiday
teɪkɪn ði `əʊn(n)i kᴧpl ə wiks ˎhɒlədeɪ
Notice that the /ŋ/ of 'taking'
has been converted to a dental [n̪] by
assimilation to the following /ð/. It doesnt sound at all as if she's
using the casual or upper-class-old-fashoned or regional version which
has a normal clear /n/. A weakform appears at 'only' caused
by omission of the /l/ of its strongform. This is int·resting as yet
another piece of evidence agenst LPD's stubborn refusal to
recognise this weakform as a non-regional usage and CEPD's sim·lar
aloofness in its regard. It's also further evidence that weakforms can
be accented (I'm not quite cert·n that its /n/ isnt actually very rapidly
geminated) contrary to the frequent assertions of some authorities.
Practicly all GB speakers use an ell-less weakform frequently but
no-one uses it at the end of a rhythm phrase.
they have all the year
| ðeɪ hav `ɔːl ðə `ˏjɜː [jɪː]
The word 'year' here is utter·d with such a close monophthongal
value that it's difficult to decide whether the speaker's target is
/ɜː/ or an allophone of /ɪə/.
with rain like this all the time.
wɪd ˈreɪn | laɪk `ðɪs ɔːl lə taɪm.
The final consonant of the word 'with' here has no friction and doesnt sound exactly
dental so it's not properly representable as either /ð/ or /d/
but we've shown it as the latter to reflect its slight abnormality.
At 'all the' we have a 'post' assimilation with the influence of the
final /l/ of 'all' converting the canonical /ð/ of the definite article
'the' into an ell. This is a very common type of assimilation which didnt apparently
receive acknowledgment in a textbook until the 2003 first edition of
the Collins-Mees Practical Phonetics etc.
5.— Too true. Fancy dragging a clutch of bawling kids
́tuː ˎtruː. ́fӕnsi `drӕgɪŋ ə ˎklᴧʧ ə ˈbɔːlɪŋ ˎkɪdz
to some bleak seaside place only to be trapped
tə sᴧm ˈbˑlik ˈsisaɪd ˎpleɪs | əʊni tə bi `trӕpt |
Note the IPA half-long mark (ˑ) after the /b/ of 'bleak' and the
absence of the full-length 'colon' at the /ˈsi/ of 'seaside'. There's
another ell-less weakform at 'only to be trapped' — this time by the
other speaker.
in some dingy boarding house.
ɪn sᴧm `-dɪnʤi| ˎbɔːdɪŋ haʊs.
The tone-sign `- before /'dɪnʤi/ indicates a fall from high restricted to ending mid.
6.—Hmm! That's why those awful holiday camps
[h`mː] ðats | waɪ ðəʊz `ɔːfl `hɒlədeɪ ˏkamps |
are so popular, no doubt.
ə səʊ `pɒpjələ nəʊ daʊt.
7.— Mm! However dragooned you are
[ ˈmˎm] haʊ ́`evə drə`guːnd ju ˏɑː|
at least there's bags of
ət ˈlist | ðəz ˈbaːgz | əv
indoor entertainment for the whole family.
`ɪndɔːr entəˏteɪnmənt | fə ðə həʊl `faːmli.
Note the emphatic Climb-Fall tone (moving from Mid to High to Low) at
'However'. Also later the (perfectly normal-sounding) withdrawal of the
'blanket' length colon at 'least' but the length colons added at 'bags'
and 'family'. Contrast these with short 'ashes' in 'fancy' and 'dragging'
at
Turn 5. These lengthened forms are as a rule completely accepted as
within the range of Gen·ral British usages but in fact such things are
markers of specificly southeastern affiliations. Cf our Blogs 144 and 360.
Blog 449 | The 10th of May 2013 |
Recently our fellow bloggist on linguistic matters, Graham Pointon, under the heading
Journalistic naïvety, or malice?
remarked that the presenters of the 9 am BBC Radio 4 chat-type program
they call 'Saturday Live', by name Sian /ʃɑːn/ Williams and Richard
Coles, revealed that they had consulted a "professor of applied linguistics"... who told them that .. the Americans had it right, pronouncing “Derby” to rhyme with “herby”, we British being wrong to call it ‘darby’.
Graham commented "I do not
believe that any professor of applied linguistics can possibly have
said anything so crass. Presumably (s)he was asked which was correct,
and .. had answered fully, saying that the older
pronunciation was /ˈdɜːrbi/, but that the pronunciation had changed in
Britain, while it had remained unchanged in the States, and I expect
that (s)he went on to say that this didn’t make either of them wrong,
but just different. Journalists are never happy with this, and
invariably extrapolate that “older” means “more correct” ".
The academic questioned, one's inclined to imagine, must've requested
that their name shd not be mentioned becoz of being embarrassed at
having to give an ans·er to such a naïve question. If that ans·er was,
as Graham suggested might be the case, saying that the older pronunciation was /ˈdɜːrbi/,
it was too simple an explanation by a long way. The fact is that during
the later
Middle English period and even until about the middle of the
eighteenth century many words with er had their spellings converted to ar.
This obvi·sly reflected the fact that numbers of speakers had been changing the vowel to something much
more open. Some old and new spellings survived simultaneously in
competition. Only
one pair has still not resolved the contest yet: we still have shard and sherd
with the newer and earlier spellings and both with the same meaning,
tho one rather than the other seems to be taking over in cert·n
contexts. A
pair which have developed strikingly diff·rent meanings (in ways too
complicated to discuss here) are person and parson.
It's not very difficult to see why a number of cases have developed of
the undesirable kind I labelled 'Grapho-Phonemic Mis-Co·ordinations' when I made them the
subject of my earlier Blog 153.
For various reasons, ranging from the sentimental to the legal, people
have long been reluctant to change the spellings of their own names or
those of the places in which they live, while yet not resisting joining
in sound changes that had taken place. Many misco·ordinations of
spellings with sounds have been the common consequences. Various
placenames have
reflected this phenomenon besides Derby, notably Berkshire and Hertford. Some more minor ones include Berkeley, Clerkenwell and Cherwell. People with the surname Bertie
are listed in CEPD and LPD as most offen pronouncing it as /bɑːti/.
Something apparently the same may be sed of owners of the surname Hervey. In some cases one spelling survives as a surname as with the second of each of the pairs hermitage and Armitage, merchant and Marchant, farmer and Fermor. In such cases, tho the new ar form with its value /ɑː/ occurs in one version, in the other the old er spelling persists but its sound has been subsequently regularised to what's become the much later value for er /ɜː/. In one case, there is a contrast of spelling tho no audible one, clerk and Clarke
— that's to say in British usage tho there is an audible diff·rence in
America. There the common noun has had its strest vowel regularised to /ɜː(r)/ as has the proper noun Berkeley.
Clerk is not quite the only common noun to have this kind of mismatch between spelling and sound: sergeant
is another; and we may note the comparable heart, hearth and hearken by contrast with the more regular ear, clear, dear, fear, near, spear, year or early, earn, earth, dearth, heard, hearse, learn, search. Pairs of names of the same origin the latter of which doesnt preserve the older er spelling may be seen in Berkeley and Barclay, Bernard and Barnard, Kerr and Carr, Derbyshire and Darbishire, Derby and Darby, Gerard and Garrard, Gerald and Jarrold, Herd and Hurd, Herbert and Harbert, Herriot and Harriott, Hervey and Harvey, Gervase and Jarvis, Perks and Parks, Perkins and Parkins, Perrot and Parrot, Perry and Parry, Verney and Varney etc.
In the same way as we've seen writers preferring to continue to use the older
spelling long after they'd adopted the later pronunciation, as with Derby,
we can detect the same thing happening from evidence provided by various poets'
rhymes. For example, among many others we may note that Spenser rhymed convert with heart and that Shakespeare (in
his Sonnet Number 17) rhymed deserts with parts and (in his Sonnet Number 72) desert with impart. Milton rhymed earth and hearth. We can see that Pope pronounced reserve as 'resarve' from his rhyming it with starve.
An odd survival is the form varsity which is a reduction of *univarsity a form of which there is no record in OED of past spellings of at the entry 'university'. This is regarded as only a slang or colloquial expression except in its use in the compound 'varsity match' for a sporting contest between the universities of Oxford and Cambridge. Two other forms that also survive only as slang or regional dialect and praps more in the US than over here are the variant larn of learn (to 'larn' someone is to teach them 'a thing or two') and the form varmint of vermin — the later of which has oddly acquired an excrescent final t. Also chiefly US is the regional interjection Massy! This derives from mercy via marcy. For the further development involved here compare hoss from horse and cuss from curse.
Blog 448 | The 1st of May 2013 |
My brief definition of a weakform "an alternant form of a word so reduced in its articulation that it consists of a different set of phonemes" has to be understood to be synchronic so that it has to be co-existent with and to alternate with a current unreduced form at least in respect of an individual speaker's idiolect. Thus items like aboard from onboard derived from elisions in the far distant past are not appropriate for this series.
We de·lt with weakforms of any in Blog 436 but not with its compounds then. Here are some:
anybody The form /`enibɒdi/ has alternants in which schwas replace the strong third vowel /ɒ/ giving /`enibədi/ or the second vowel /i/ giving /`enəbɒdi/ or both such replacements. Much less offen the second vowel may be elided giving /`enbɒdi/ etc. These alternants are weakforms for speakers who use the first form in accented occurrences but any other than the last may be an individual speaker's regular form.
anyone The form /`eniwᴧn/ is the usual GB form of this word when free from rhythmic pressure but in some rhythmic contexts as in eg anyone `else it may easily take a weakform /eniwən/ or more casually /enwən/. The CEPD listing of an alternant /ˈen.i.wən/ without any qualifying comment on its use is prob·bly on a par with its other questionable admission of a regionally markt alternant /ˈen.i.wɒn/ for the word.
anything The strongform /`eniθɪŋ/ has the weak alternants /`enəθɪŋ/ and less commonly /`enθɪŋ/.
anyway Besides the obvious variants /`enəweɪ/ and occasionally the schwa-elided /`enweɪ/ this word has the further developments from this last version /emweɪ, `ẽweɪ/ and the denasalised /`eweɪ/ which are not at all as unusual as people may imagine. There's a nice example to be found of the [ẽ] variant to be heard in the penultimate 25th episode of the famous 1967 BBCtv serial The Forsyte Saga fom the actor Peter Copley in his role of the vicar Hilary Forsyte still fortunately available on a DVD.
anywhere In cert·n rhythmic situations the regular form /`eniwɛː/ may give way to a form ending with schwa inste·d of its usual vowel eg as in anywhere else as /eniwər `els/.
assembly As /ə`semli/ this
example may stand for various weakforms which arise from assimilations
and elisions etc like the medial /b/ from the sequence /-mbl-/here.
Such cases are at best borderline admissions to the category of
weakform words and are mainly not included in the present listing.
been This has two GB weakforms /bin/ (ord·nrily recorded as such in transcriptions which alternate length-markt and not-so-markt versions of the street vowel) and, less offen, /bɪn/. For a minority of GB speakers and the majority of GA speakers /bɪn/ isnt a weakform but their usual form.
between I suspect that I'm not the on·y speaker who occasion·y in a relaxt moment uses the weakform 'tween' inst·ed of the more orthodox 'between' but it's the kind of thing that it's not easy to collect data on. See also our Blog 441.
but This has only one commom weakform /bət/ but it does have an occasional form /pt/ sentence initially before vowels as mentioned above in our Blog 441.
(by had the whole of our Blog 422 devoted to it.)
(can was described in Blog 425.)
certainly Like only,
this is very, if not most, offen he·rd in the conversations of GB
speakers without its orthographic /l/ tho people usually do have a
strongform with the /l/. It is therefore usually /`sɜːtn̩i/ with a
syllabic /n/ which easily loozes its syllabicity in various rhythmic
contexts.
clothes This has the common weakform /kləʊz/.
day This has no weakforms but its compounds are intended to be be de·lt with at 'Monday'.
did This has the casual weakforms /d, dəd/ and the rare type /dd/ eg in D'you get those yesterday?, What /dəd i/ did he say? and How /dd/ it go? Unlike CEPD (and unsurprisingly ODP), which give no weakforms for did, LPD has "occasional weakforms § dəd, d" and a cross-reference to a note at its entry "-'d". The sign "§" to suggest that it's regionally markt seems unjustified here.
directly This adverb, in the sense of 'immediately' has the casual form /drekli/. LPD records this, saying prob·bly justifiably, that it's "becoming old-fashioned".
do LPD surprisingly gives no
weakform /du/ for this word but gives /dʊ/, before /də/, which prompts
one to wonder where the EAL user is recommended to employ it relative
to /də/. What /dʊ/ they say? for example wd seem to be, if clearly pronounced, inclined to sound a little
old-fashioned and/or precious. An occasional minor form not listed is
/dw/ which can occur in eg Do I have to? /dw aɪ `haf ˏtuː/.
doing This has the
weakform /dwɪŋ/ in relaxt styles, eg /wə `nɒt dwɪŋ mʌʧ təˏnaɪt / We're not doing much tonight. Some speakers, including the Queen, have a
conversational form /`də.ɪŋ/ with an accented schwa. This is praps a good
example of 'UGB'.
don't Like all -nt forms, this offen occurs with no final /t/ except before pause. Particu·ly in the collocation don't know the accented-schwa form /`də nəʊ/ frequently occurs in conversational style, sometimes represented in writing as dunno, eg in the apologetic opening /aɪ `də nəʊ baʊ `ˏʧuː.../ I don't know about you... to be he·rd eg from the well-known broadcaster John Tusa.
everybody Rather surprisingly neither LPD nor CEPD lists the weakform /`evribədi/.
everyone Less surprisingly,
neither LPD nor CEPD lists the weakform /`evriwən/. This seems to be mainly
limited to occurring unaccented immediately before an accented word
such as in /evriwən `els/ everyone else [evriwə`nels].
February Prob·bly most GB
speakers have a trisyllabic strongform for this word but I'm not
convinced it's /`feb.ru(ə).ri/ as LPD and CEPD seem to suggest. It may
well be /`febr̩i/ but /`febjəri/ is also common. Less so are
/`febjueri/ and /`febjuəri /. All of these may be weakforms for speakers who may in
self-conscious contexts opt for something like /`febru.əri/.
for This extremely common preposition has, besides its usual /fɔː/, as LPD sez, for "some speakers" a prevocalic-only further strongform /fɒr/ used not quite as limitedly as LPD suggests ie only before her, him, it or us.
Forsyte Saga 1967 has /fɒr/ by M. Mont in antepenult episode. For
example it may occur in /`fɒr ɪg`ˏzɑːmpl/ or in /`fɒr ə ˏtaɪm... For a time...
CEPD seems to regard this usage as too recessive to be worth including.
The weakform /fə(r)/ may be accented eg in /`fər ə ˎməʊmənt
ət ˏliːst/ For a moment at least. It very offen loozes its vowel, its potential /r/ remaining, before vocalic sounds eg in /`ɪn fr ə`ˏpeni/ In for a penny... An /r/-less version as in /`fɒ ðə ˎˏməʊmənt/ For the moment...
Blog 447 | The 27th of April 2013 |
It was sad to hear of the recent death of our colleague John
Baldwin. He was born on the 3rd of June in 1935 in east London at
West Ham and died on the 14th of this month. He was a fine scholar and
a modest, agreeable person who was, I'm sure, liked by everybody who
met him. After obtaining an MA (on Russian consonant clusters) at the
UCL (University College London) Department of Phonetics and,
subsequently acquiring a doctorate, he became appointed to its staff on
which he served for many years.
Besides Russian and German he was particu·ly int·rested in cert·n
languages of southeastern Europe including Turkish. He was from 1966 a
member of the International Phonetic Association for many years, during
ten of which he served on its governing Council. He was a contributor
to the Association's Journal, writing for it a number of reviews and
articles including ones on such topics as the glottal stop in Turkish
and ejectives in Georgian and most importantly a substantial two-part
'Formal Analysis of the Intonation of Modern Colloquial Russian'. He
was also a great enthusiast for and expert on the folk music of the
Balkans, for years performing in a band that specialised in it.
One of his last publications was an eight-page article contributed at
my invitation for inclusion in a book which I edited in 1995, Studies in General and English Phonetics: Essays in Honour of Professor J. D. O'Connor,
the lecturer, incidentally, who supervised John's MA thesis. It
appeared in the section of that book entitled 'The Phonetics of
Mother-tongue English'. He chose to call it, with characteristic
modesty, A 'tenny' rate, but it was no lightweight piece of scholarship. Its subject was 'The
process whereby a consonant or more than one consonant at the end of
one word is transferred in connected speech to the beginning of the
next word if it has a vowel onset'.
This drew for its subject matter not a little from the kinds of
observations that were to be a very important feature of the next
remarkable stage of his career.
The nineteen fifties and sixties saw great acceleration in the spread
of the use of first telephones and latterly sound recording equipment.
By the end of that period inexpensive and easily operated compact
cassette tape recorders had become widely available to the general
public. All emergency services had become equipped with large drum tape
recorders that were taking down ev·ry call to the '999' emergency
services round the clock. Circumstances like these led to a rather
sudden new impetus in a field that became known as 'forensic
phonetics'. In the early sixties the late Dennis Fry, occupant of a UCL
Chair of Experimental Phonetics, as John remarked, "was involved in a number of civil and criminal cases",
but things were to develop very rapidly when, in the later sixties
police and lawyers took to appealing to the UCL Department for help
with criminal identification and other matters. John turned out to be
the staff member willing to undertake that sort of work.
For the next twenty-five years he never lookt back. He was almost
continuously in demand. He enjoyed driving himself to courts and legal
chambers all over the country to a wide variety of criminal cases and
other matters. In the course of these activities we used from time to time to meet up with each other. He described
them in a fascinating book he called Forensic Phonetics
publisht in 1990 along with Peter French who provided for insertion in
it a twenty-page article on a division of the science into which John
never himself felt inclined to set foot, viz Chapter 3, entitled
'Acoustic Phonetics'. Professor French is these days in demand
worldwide and the leading practitioner in forensic phonetics in this
country. In the so-called 'Home Page' major division of this website
there is a copy, its Section 12 Item 6, of a review of John's book that
I contributed to The Times
newspaper. Readers may like to know that Section 6 of that 'Home Page'
is devoted to my own experiences in the field. Baldwin, French and I
were all founder members in 1991 of the International Association for
Forensic Phonetics. In the years of his retirement John became the
victim of an unfortunate increasingly debilitating condition. He'll be
remembered with affection by all those who knew him.
Blog 446 | The 25th of April 2013 |
A welcome appearance earlier this year has been the arrival of a
further-enlarged new (third) edition from Routledge of the Collins and
Mees Practical Phonetics and Phonology.
With thirty new pages it's now a hundred pages longer than its closest
rival. It offers lots of very genuinely practical guidance on the
pronunciation of British English but also a great deal more than just
that. Even tho its main target is the user of English as an extra
language, one can well imagine it functioning as at least an
auxiliary initial text for a student of general phonetics or as a
serious phonetic component of a course in any language. Students of the
very numerously spoken languages Spanish, French, German, Italian,
Polish and Japanese are especially lucky in being supplied with brief
'concise overviews' of the phonetic features of each language complete
with vowel diagrams. These are included, of course, not only for the
light
they shine on the problems their native speakers have in acquiring
spoken English but for the similar or identical problems that are
experienced by numerous speakers of other languages. In addition the
user will find many comments on diff·rent
difficulties experienced by countless speakers of quite other languages
than these six. They are presented along with very substantial
samplings of the
uses of English worldwide.
The total numbers of speakers of one or two of the world's languages
may be as many or more than those of English, but even so English is
the most widespread form of truly international communication that has
ever existed. The two most studied forms of English pronunciation — in
my preferred designations General American and General British — are
fortunately very comf·tably mutually intelligible and the latter is, of
course, the principal target of the major, practical, part of this book
tho American pronunciations are occasionally referred to and most of
what is de·lt with wd be equally helpful to students of American
English.
From its first edition of 2003 it has shown no hesitation in abandoning
unsatisfactory traditional terminology, replacing the outdated
'Received Pronunciation' with its own much better term 'Non-Regional
Pronunciation' (abbreviation NRP). Its list of phonemes very reasonably
avoids the recessive diphthong /ɛə/ in favour of the simple vowel /ɛː/
also adopted in the most recent newly-devised pronunciation dictionary
(the one brau·t out by OUP in 2001) and used in non-EFL OUP
dictionaries starting in 1993. It'll soon be seen to be favoured
in the most respected of all the descriptions of English pronunciation.
Other innovations include the rejection of the Latinate terms
'regressive' and 'progressive' for which it uses the simpler more
transparent terms 'leading' and 'lagging' for assimilations.
Its coverage of other varieties than the one being taught is remarkable
for its non-trivial amounts of recorded examples. Its claim to be a
practical course is well borne out by the liberal supplies of exercises
it contains, including plenty of passages for phonemic transcription,
tho I tend to wonder whether taking ev·ry one of the dozen of them from
Alice in Wonderland shou·dnt
be reconsidered in the future. The ten extra ones on its website are
more varied if still mainly non-conversational. Phonemic transcriptions
are prob·bly the surest way of alerting students to the importance of
acquiring proficiency in using weakforms and contractions, topics which
are very well explained in the text. The difficult choice of which
weakforms to recommend has been judiciously kept to a minimum tho
I'm afraid I can't concur with excluding the word but
from that minimum. Transcriptions are required to include marking of
'sentence stress' and 'intonation groups' but there are regrettably no
exercises requiring intonations to be shown. Intonation is the one
topic of the book that I shdve liked to see given more attention —
altho it's at least as well treated here as in any of its rivals I know
of.
A notable feature is the rich selection of excellent illustrations with
as many as a hundred figures including vocal tract and mouth drawings,
many outstandingly effective vowel diagrams, and maps of world
Englishes and accent varieties etc. The accomp·nying CD has over three
quarters of an hour of materials including twenty-five absorbing
examples of accents ranging from an amusing sample of 'Traditional RP'
to two dozen accent types from all around the world. These are each
accompanied by a full text of what's spoken and descriptions of 'salient
phonetic features'. There are also well over a hundred varied items
labelled 'Activities'. There are even illustrations of how English
sounded in past centuries, and some challenging items called Accent
Detective Work.
The book is rounded off with ten substantial int·resting extracts, from
works on various aspects of phonetics, by such authorities as Daniel
Jones, John Wells, Peter Ladefoged and David Crystal, all followed by
suggestions and questions. As well as its disc, the book has a valuable
associated Website containing keys to the Activity Exercises and transcriptions that are
in the book, additional exercises including the extra transcription
passages on·y on the website, a version of the Glossary with a
'flashcard' facility that enables students to quiz themselves on their
knowledge of its contents, concise audio files of native-speaker
illustrations of the vowels and consonants of the six languages we
mentioned and numbers of direct links to other websites containing
further resources. This brief account doesn't do justice to the rich
amounts of valu·ble materials contained in this surely unique book.
It's also a very fine piece of book production with clear, well set out
text. In spite of the difficulty for the printer of so much of it in
phonetic symbols, I've scrutinised just about ev·ry page of it and on·y
spotted one very minor misprint — and that's not in the printed book but the website. It even has a strikingly handsome
cover. In view of all it offers, it seems pri·tty reasonably priced at
£20.99 in paperback.
Blog 445 | The 23rd of April 2013 |
Blog 444 | The 23rd of April 2013 |
I've been wond·ring if some readers might like to hear a little more
about the Apple-Mac text-to-speech facility I mentioned the other day
in my Blog 440. It provides synthetic voices to ree·d out any text one
selects. To avail oneself of them one simply has to click on a toolbar item
entitled 'Speak Selection'. The test sentences illustrating them
demonstrate the distinct advantage of having the facility but, at the
same time, how definitely limited is its capability of providing really
natural-sounding delivery. It has sample sentences that can be reached
by going thru the sequence 'Finder → System Preferences → System → Date
& Time → Clock' and clicking on 'Announce the time → Customize
Voice'. There you can set speed of utterance, volume and voice choice.
You can chooze between five male and five female ones. One of the male
ones is the default you get if you opt to 'Use System Voice'.
This default one may be sampled by clicking on 'Play' — when you hear it say / 'moʊst `ˏpipl | `rekᵻgˏnaɪz mi | baɪ maɪ `vɔɪs
/. This sounds impressively natural. However, at a normal pace it praps
tends to sound just a little nerdy. They call it 'Alec'. Played slow it
still sounds natural but for a drugged or drowsy or long-suffering
person. Played fast it also sounds natural but of course hurried. I
refrained from testing what my impressions might've been had I upped
the volume from the degree of 2 in a series of ten.
For the quarter-hourly reminders of the time I'm pleased to receive I
chooze normal-rate, lowish-volume announcements by the light female
voice they call 'Vicki'. When I ask her to give me a sample of her
speaking she sez / ɪzənt ɪt `naɪs | tə hӕv ə kəm`pjuwdə | ðӕt wɪl `tɔk tə ˈjuw
/. This is pretty respectable but I feel the schwa rather than a
syllabic /n/ in the first word sounds a tiny bit unnatural. I notice
with int·rest that the t of computer is cert·nly an American [d] whereas the non occurrence of rhoticity at its final -er sounds sort-of 'Atlantic' rather than General American. The very weak vowel value of the two occurrences of the preposition to is very natural sounding. So is the schwa for the indefinite article at a computer. On the contrary, the two-words sequence that will which wd normally combine to produce the contraction that'll /ðətl/ goes to the other extreme ie as / ðӕt wɪl /. The last tone — on the word you
— sounds eccentricly like the moment in the stage or screen
entertainment they call a 'musical' when a speaker suddenly bursts into
song. This is because the naturally expected
tone, after the previous fall, in this sort of phrase wd be a rise. And it's on·y the most
whimsical or dreamy or inattentive or crazy speaker who'd be very likely to end a
sentence — if they're not saying goodbye to you — on a high level tone.
The other voice options are not all demonstrated with the same sentence. Some use / aɪ ˈʃʊr ˈlaɪk `biɪŋ | ɪnsaɪd ðɪs ˈfӕnsi kəm`pjuwdər /. A dozy youngster called Junior sez / maɪ ˈfeɪvərᵻt ˎfuwd | ɪz ə `pitsə
/ not as clearly as the others. A deep-voiced guy sounding faintly like
Kissinger sez very unclearly something one has partly to guess at / ðᵻ `sᴧm| əv ðᵻ `skwɛrz | əv ðə `lek [sic] əv ðə ˈraɪt `traɪˏӕŋgl| ɪz `ikwəl | tə ðə skwɛr | əv ðə haɪ`pɑtn̩uwz /. Finally 'Princess' sez in a convincing child voice / wen aɪ ˈgroʊ ˈᴧp| aɪm ˈgoʊɪŋ | tə ˎbij | ə `saɪəntɪst
/. There're actually fourteen more 'novelty' voices available including
'whisper, frog-in-the-throat, hysterical, deranged, pipe-organ, bad
news, good news, bells, boing' and 'bubbles' — rather few of which I've
investigated I'm afraid.
Blog 443 | The 14th of April 2013 |
It’s rather odd to find oneself impelled to write about a matter which
one feels in truth is not worth discussing at all on account of its
extreme triviality. I refer to the question of what proportion of
the English-speaking population of ‘Britain’ (praps one might better
say of the United Kingdom ie England, Wales and Scotland) speaks
unadulterated RP. I refer to
it not out of enthusiasm for the topic but from exasperation at the way
it’s seemed be taken ridiculously seriously by surprisingly numerous commentators.
The
person who has innocently led to my irritation is the distinguisht
dialectologist Professor Peter Trudgill. He is very prob·bly irritated
in the same way as I am by seeing such a minor observation of his
trotted out as if it were of prime significance. Indeed it must be
highly disagreeable for him to find that, with all the admirable work
he has done, he is famous perhaps more than anything else, at least among much
of the phonetics community, for what he may well have originally
considered essentially little more than an inconsequential aside. After
all, in adapting his 1971 doctoral thesis for publication he didnt
choose to include even any reference at all to the notorious ‘3%’ which is the subject of these present remarks.
However that may be, his article ‘Received Pronunciation: Sociolinguistic Aspects’ of 2001 began: ‘An
often cited statistic has it that in Britain RP speakers constitute
only 3% of the population. When this statistic first became commonplace
in the sociolinguistics literature, it was not unusual for people to
dispute it’. It was disappointing that at this point he neither
provided any details nor even cited any source of such criticisms. At
any rate, he continued: ‘Perhaps, therefore, it will be as well to discuss where this statistic came from. The guilty party was myself. I popularised [sic] the 3% figure in Trudgill (1974)’.
Here again we find rather tantalisingly that, so far from his putting
the statistic into circulation in that book — whose title was The social differentiation of English in Norwich
— he made no reference of any sort whatever to it anywhere in its text.
In making the above remark, he’d apparently had in mind Chapter 15 of
his previous unpublisht work of 1971 from whose title the 1974
abbreviated version was unfortunately not differentiated.
Anyway, he continued to explain how his sociolinguistic dialect study of the city of Norwich, ‘some of the findings of which were presented in Trudgill (1974)’, was based for the most part on interviews with a statisticly strictly random sample of fifty people taken from the population of that city. The article in which he made these remarks may be seen online here. Out of this sample he reported only one individual as judged to be an ‘RP’ speaker. He commented ‘In other words, the evidence [a slightly more suitable term might’ve been ‘inference’] from my random sample was that the population of Norwich contained only 2% of RP speakers.’ In ‘considering to what extent [he] could generalise from this finding to Britain as a whole’ he took a number of stated factors into careful consideration. ‘In the end, [he] decided that 3% was approximately correct’ as an extrapolation from this by way of estimating the total number of ‘RP’ speakers in ‘Britain’ at the time.
Even if I accepted this conclusion it wd make no diff·rence to my impatience with the importance various people seem to’ve been attaching to it. I tend to find myself pondering uneasily on certain aspects of the investigation. It was described as based on fifty subjects selected on the basis of sixty interviews ten of which were carried out by a collaborator. One thing I can’t help wondering is whether someone other than Trudgill might’ve disagreed with the judgment that the single individual in question fitted into the category of ‘RP speaker’. An objection might’ve been raised in various ways — possibly in categories other than the strictly segmental ones to which Trudgill almost exclusively devoted his thesis. There’s not even any single mention of prosodic features anywhere in the whole of the printed book, altho it’s a pleasure to acknowledge that the very condensed account of ‘articulatory settings’ is admirably covered in the relatively limited space accorded to it.
In his various writings he regularly insists, as for instance in an article of 2002, that ‘It takes only one non-RP feature for a speaker not to be RP’.
It’s my impression that very many of my colleagues in the phonetic
sphere have not at all been in confident agreement about assigning
individual speakers to such a category. Any reader who cares to look at
my Blog 360 will find clear evidence of far from total harmony in one
area — namely between the most distinguisht lexicographers of ‘RP’.
What has struck me as rather remarkable is how ready various scholars
have been to repeat the 3% comment tho no-one seems to have considered
replicating the investigation. When I consider that this Trudgill
estimate is the only one of its kind that I, at least, know of, I read
with misgivings remarks like ‘Phoneticians
in Britain generally agree that RP is spoken by about 3 per cent,
possibly slightly more, of the population of Britain’ and ‘usually estimated as being used by somewhere between 3–5% of the population’ both by noted scholars. These suggest to me that indiff·rence may be being unwarrantably interpreted as approval.
Seeing extrapolation from a figure of one person
in fifty in the
present case to such percentages of ‘RP’ speakers in the whole of the
Great Britain (unless one shd be saying the United Kingdom)
population, what percentage might’ve been suggested if the number of
‘RP’ speakers had not been one but nil. Cou·d it’ve been that Britain
contained something between one and minus two percent of ‘RP’ speakers?
But seriously, exactly how many speakers of it
might exist and with
what degree of ‘purity’, is an absurd matter to focus attention upon in
regard to the relatively least locally affiliated variety of UK English
speech. The only truly significant consideration is not the numbers of
individuals belonging to the group but the disproportionately large
influence that this relatively small group undou·tedly has had on the
perceptions and practices of so many other users of any significance in
the
British-English-speaking world. Indications of what very large numbers
of speakers exist who may have ‘impurities’ and yet function as virtual
‘RP’ speakers are readily available. Now that’d provide a really
int·resting percentage if it were possible for someone to succeed in
estimating it in a respectably credible way. We’re reminded daily of
this, if we can be bothered to entertain the thaut, when we — as so
offen — hear from a loudspeaker a not already known voice that
may present us with an ‘impurity’ but only after sometimes minutes of
careful liss·ening out for such a thing. Until that moment is reached
we
are unaware, if we accept the Trudgill criterion, whether the speaker
is to be categorised as an ‘RP’ user or
not. [Some very minor emendations were made at 26 May 2017.]
Blog 442 | The 10th of April 2013 |
The great British phonetician Henry Sweet was the scholar who at the
age of forty publisht in 1885 the very first ever book on the phonetics
of English specificly written for non-native-speaking users. He had
already publisht a number of brilliant works notably on the history of
English and including his epoch-making 1877 Handbook of Phonetics.
His supreme achievements had been recognised by the
Paris-based Association Internationale Phonétique (alias IPA) by their
electing him their Honorary President, a post which he held for the rest of his life. At the age of 55 (when Daniel
Jones was 19) Sweet provided the Association's modest periodical Le Maître Phonétique (the precursor to the IPA's Journal) with the paragraph that we're indetted to Kraut for relaying to us in his blog of the fourth of April 2013.
Its 148 words in eighteen lines employed a selection of symbols that was very similar to those he'd used for that 1885 Elementarbuch des Gesprochenen Englisch. That book was so well received that Oxford University Press had him follow it up in 1890 with a translation entitled A Primer of Spoken English. The 1900 transcription's editor's title for the paragraph was simply Anglais du Sud
meaning Southern British English — an undesirably ambiguous term
especially when some people, at least in those days, were given to
using 'North Britain' to mean Scotland. (Sweet had been dead fourteen
years by the time Jones started promoting the term 'received
pronunciation' he'd taken up from A. J. Ellis).
We notice immediately that the slight diphthongality of the two closest
vowels in their most characteristic realisations was taken into account
by Sweet in his pref·rence to represent them not as /iː/ and /uː/ but
as the glide-ending diphthongs /ij/ and /uw/. In Jones's opinion the
diphthongal variant was not as typical as the simple one — hence his
pref·rence for [iː] and [uː] with their length marks rather than glide
symbols to represent these phonemes. Various Americans including
Leonard Bloomfield favoured such final-glide representations.
Next we notice that, tho the ship
/ʃɪp/ vowel is shown as /i/, there're quite a number of occurrences of
an undotted [ı] used in unaccented syllables (of various values) eg in the before any vowels and also in all three unaccented syllables of encourages
/ın`kᴧrıʤız/. Additionally, he used this [ı] where in today's most
popular notation for General British pronunciations [i] wd be used in
words like association /əˈsəʊsı`eɪʃn/, and study
/`stᴧdi/. This dotless [ı] had not, I believe, in fact been accorded
recognition as an item of the Association's official alphabet at the
time and has in fact never been so subsequently. He also used it to
begin the diphthong we now show as /ɪə/.
We see that, in a manner that Jones was later to follow, he represented
the most open of our GB rounded vowels, the phoneme of words like clock with the symbol /ɔ/ (unlike our now usual Gimsonian style /ɒ/) and the mid one of words like jaw
with the same letter plus length sign as /ɔː/ — which our Gimsonian
practice matches. It's rather int·resting that at the unaccented
occurrences of the words or and your
before following vowels in lines 1 and 9 he reveals his choice of the
/ɒ/ phoneme. Neither of the two principal pronunciation dictionaries,
the Wells LPD and the Roach-&-co CEPD, refers to the existence of
this /ɒr/ weakform of or but
I can't help wondering if it's still being used by numbers of people
without its being noticed. I wonder how many people are aware of the
existence of a similar weakform /fɒr/ of the preposition for.
I'm not surprised that this isnt listed as a third weakform by CEPD.
Jones's and Gimson's EPDs always listed /fɒr/ as an "occasional strong
[sic] form before vowels" but it's not easy to estimate just how
frequently it's used today. I use it — but then I'm very old. Anyway,
I'm happy to find it noted in LPD tho I dont accept that it now only
occurs before the four pronouns her, him, it and us.
This is prob·bly not the place to go into the reasons why I regard
/fɒr/ as better termed a 'weakform' than a 'strong form'. No dictionary
has any record of any surviving use of a weakform /jɒr/ but I wonder
how many people carry on using it today unnoticed: I sh·ll have to
lissen to myself more carefully. We neednt dou·t that it was a usage of
Henry Sweet's. Rather similarly one notices Sweet's pref·rence for what
is now written in Gimsonian symbols as /ʧʊldrən/ for the plural of child.
It's understandable that CEPD shd not bother to show this variant tho
it was always included in the EPD of Jones's day but I'm glad to
welcome LPD's inclusion of it. Indeed I regret LPD's omission of the
other regularly included Jones variant with a syllabic /l/ in its first
syllable which I fancy to be as frequently used as the /ʊ/ one.
These ar·nt the only usages Sweet displayed that've become
oldfashioned. Few people use their /əʊ/ phoneme in the first syllable
of phonetics today and, of
course, since Gimson revised the Jones EPD we've mostly adopted his
preferred notation rather than the /ou/ that Jones always used in it
for that phoneme. Praps the most numerous diff·rences from Sweet's
usages seen in this paragraph are our latterday pref·rences for /ə/
where he used [ı] to be seen at society, nationalities, utilising, foreign, possible, alphabet and system.
Alongside these schwas, some of which didnt become predominant until
the second half of the last century, it's int·resting to see that he'd
already adopted one in the second syllable of representation. His dropping of the former schwa from between the /n/ and the /r/ of generally also seems rather modern for his day.
Sweet used a number of diff·rent sets of transcriptional symbols for
English at various times. This was one of his last ones but not the
last. Seven years later, for his book The Sounds of English,
he employed yet another, rather more complicated, symbol set five years
before he died in 1912. Anyway, this 1900 choice was evidently the
Sweet transcription that Daniel Jones liked best. He adopted it
unchanged for all his major books on English with the sole minor
exceptions of [ij, ı, uw & ıə] giving [iː, i, e, , ɑː, ɒ, ɔː, ʊ,
uː, ᴧ, əː, ə; ei, ou, ai, au, ɔi, iə, ɛə, ʊə]. A totally trivial
diff·rence after 1927 was due to the fact that the IPA decided to
replace its earlier official sign for 'primary stress' [ ́] with
the present [ˈ].